A Dominance Model for the Calculation of Decoy Products in Recommendation Environments

Recommender systems support internet users in finding and identifying products and services suiting their individual demands (e.g.: digital cameras, financial services). Although recommender systems already offer mechanisms which alleviate the comparison of different suitable products (e.g. product lists, comparison pages) users usually have difficulties in decision making and finding the optimal option. Persuasive mechanisms can be used in such situations for underlining product differences and reinforcing confidence in the users’ own decision. This leads to an increase of the trust level and supports the decision making process. Especially theories concerning user behaviour in buying situations constitute great potential for persuasion in recommender systems. It has been shown that the user’s perception of the value of a certain product is highly influenced by the context (i.e. the set of presented products in the choice set). Well-known context effects are the asymmetric dominance effect, the attraction effect, or the compromise effect. This paper presents a multi-preferential and multi-alternative model (i.e. more than two product attributes and more than two products are supported) for calculating dominance values of items in choice sets and thus offers the possibility of determining the best recommendation set in a given choice situation. The performance of the model is shown by the application on empirical data (choice sets) gained by a previously conducted user study.

[1]  Xue Yang,et al.  Effects of Choice Contrast and Order Sequence on Consumer Judgment and Decision in Comparison- Shopping Assisted Environment , 2004 .

[2]  Alexander Felfernig,et al.  Persuasion in Knowledge-Based Recommendation , 2008, PERSUASIVE.

[3]  Daniel S. Levine,et al.  Multiattribute Decision Making in Context: A Dynamic Neural Network Methodology , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  Ralph Westfall,et al.  Psychological Factors in Predicting Product Choice , 1962 .

[5]  Judith Masthoff,et al.  Effective explanations of recommendations: user-centered design , 2007, RecSys '07.

[6]  M. Kenward,et al.  An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 2007 .

[7]  Ravi Dhar,et al.  Similarity in Context: Cognitive Representation and Violation of Preference and Perceptual Invariance in Consumer Choice☆ , 1996 .

[8]  Robin Burke,et al.  Knowledge-based recommender systems , 2000 .

[9]  I. Simonson,et al.  Choice Based on Reasons: The Case of Attraction and Compromise Effects , 1989 .

[10]  Candel,et al.  A Probabilistic Feature Model for Unfolding Tested for Perfect and Imperfect Nestings , 1997, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[11]  Michael J. Pazzani,et al.  Learning and Revising User Profiles: The Identification of Interesting Web Sites , 1997, Machine Learning.

[12]  Valerie J. Trifts,et al.  Consumer Decision Making in Online Shopping Environments: The Effects of Interactive Decision Aids , 2000 .

[13]  John Riedl,et al.  Explaining collaborative filtering recommendations , 2000, CSCW '00.

[14]  Gerald Häubl,et al.  Running Head : PROCESSES OF PREFERENCE CONSTRUCTION Processes of Preference Construction in Agent-Assisted Online Shopping , 2002 .

[15]  Maria Giovanna Devetag From utilities to mental models: A critical survey on decision rules and cognition in consumer choice , 1999 .

[16]  Alexander Felfernig,et al.  An Empirical Study on Consumer Behavior in the Interaction with Knowledge-based Recommender Applications , 2006, The 8th IEEE International Conference on E-Commerce Technology and The 3rd IEEE International Conference on Enterprise Computing, E-Commerce, and E-Services (CEC/EEE'06).

[17]  G. G. Stokes "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[18]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  A set-theoretic random utility model of choice behavior , 1980 .

[19]  Oded Netzer,et al.  Alternative Models for Capturing the Compromise Effect , 2004 .

[20]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[21]  Dan Ariely,et al.  Seeking Subjective Dominance in Multidimensional Space: An Explanation of the Asymmetric Dominance Effect , 1995 .

[22]  Steven Callander,et al.  Context-dependent Voting , 2006 .

[23]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choice in Context: Tradeoff Contrast and Extremeness Aversion , 1992 .

[24]  Gerhard Friedrich,et al.  An Integrated Environment for the Development of Knowledge-Based Recommender Applications , 2006, Int. J. Electron. Commer..

[25]  N. Klein,et al.  Context Effects on Effort and Accuracy in Choice: An Enquiry into Adaptive Decision Making , 1989 .

[26]  Hans Pechtl Definitions- und Wirkungsbereiche des decoy-Effekts: eine empirisch-explorative Untersuchung , 2004 .

[27]  R. Srivastava,et al.  Explaining Context Effects on Choice Using a Model of Comparative Judgment , 2000 .