Eye Behavior Associated with Internally versus Externally Directed Cognition

What do our eyes do when we are focused on internal representations such as during imagination or planning? Evidence from mind wandering research suggests that spontaneous shifts from externally directed cognition (EDC) to internally directed cognition (IDC) involves oculomotor changes indicative of visual disengagement. In the present study, we investigated potential differences in eye behavior between goal-directed forms of IDC and EDC. To this end, we manipulated the focus of attention (internal versus external) in two demanding cognitive tasks (anagram and sentence generation). IDC was associated with fewer and longer fixations and higher variability in pupil diameter and eye vergence compared to EDC, suggesting reduced visual scanning and higher spontaneous eye activity. IDC was further related to longer blinks, lower microsaccade frequency, and a lower angle of eye vergence. These latter changes appear conducive to attenuate visual input and thereby shield ongoing internal processes from external distraction. Together, these findings suggest that IDC is accompanied by characteristic eye behavior that reflects a decoupling of attention from external events and serves gating out visual input.

[1]  M. Raichle Two views of brain function , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  W. Ray,et al.  EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. , 1985, Science.

[3]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  K. Paterson,et al.  Effects of social gaze on visual-spatial imagination , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[5]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top‐down processing in a working memory task , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[6]  M. Benedek,et al.  Tackling creativity at its roots: Evidence for different patterns of EEG alpha activity related to convergent and divergent modes of task processing , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  Hans Supèr,et al.  A Role of Eye Vergence in Covert Attention , 2013, PloS one.

[8]  J. Antrobus,et al.  Looking with the mind's eye: experimental studies of ocular motility during daydreaming and mental arithmetic. , 1971, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[9]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The default network and self‐generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance , 2014, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[11]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Going AWOL in the Brain: Mind Wandering Reduces Cortical Analysis of External Events , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Julie D. Golomb,et al.  A taxonomy of external and internal attention. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[13]  Matthew L. Dixon,et al.  A framework for understanding the relationship between externally and internally directed cognition , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  L Stark,et al.  Topology of the near response triad , 1990, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[15]  J. Smallwood Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  Hong-Jin Sun,et al.  Modulation of microsaccade rate by task difficulty revealed through between- and within-trial comparisons. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[17]  C. Neuper,et al.  The creative brain: Investigation of brain activity during creative problem solving by means of EEG and FMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation , 2005, Neuron.

[19]  G. Thut,et al.  Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  K. Paterson,et al.  Effects of gaze-aversion on visual-spatial imagination. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[21]  J. Antrobus,et al.  EYE MOVEMENTS ACCOMPANYING DAYDREAMING, VISUAL IMAGERY, AND THOUGHT SUPPRESSION. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  J. Henderson,et al.  Taking a new look at looking at nothing , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  P. Silvia,et al.  For Whom the Mind Wanders, and When , 2007, Psychological science.

[24]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[25]  W. Klimesch Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  D Kahneman,et al.  Pupil Diameter and Load on Memory , 1966, Science.

[27]  Mathias Benedek,et al.  Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition , 2018 .

[28]  A. Burgess,et al.  Paradox lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations during externally vs. internally directed attention and the implications for idling and inhibition hypotheses. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[29]  S. Steinhauer,et al.  Blink before and after you think: blinks occur prior to and following cognitive load indexed by pupillary responses. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[30]  Steve Joordens,et al.  The eyes know what you are thinking: Eye movements as an objective measure of mind wandering , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  M. Benedek,et al.  EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down processing in convergent and divergent thinking , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  T. Troscianko,et al.  Effort during visual search and counting: Insights from pupillometry , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  W. Dunlap,et al.  Meta-Analysis of Experiments With Matched Groups or Repeated Measures Designs , 1996 .

[34]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[35]  M. Benedek,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Eeg Alpha Power and Creative Ideation , 2022 .

[36]  Claire Braboszcz,et al.  Oculometric variations during mind wandering , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[37]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Eye Movements During Mindless Reading , 2010, Psychological science.

[38]  Aljoscha C. Neubauer,et al.  Brain correlates underlying the generation of original ideas , 2008 .

[39]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  R. VanRullen,et al.  An oscillatory mechanism for prioritizing salient unattended stimuli , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  H. Ehrlichman,et al.  Why Do People Move Their Eyes When They Think? , 2012 .

[42]  P. Lang,et al.  Oscillatory brain activity in the alpha range is modulated by the content of word-prompted mental imagery. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Pupillometric Evidence for the Decoupling of Attention from Perceptual Input during Offline Thought , 2011, PloS one.

[44]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  Looking for Creativity: Where Do We Look When We Look for New Ideas? , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[45]  Fiona G. Phelps,et al.  Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[46]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  The Decoupled Mind: Mind-wandering Disrupts Cortical Phase-locking to Perceptual Events , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  J. Kounios,et al.  Sudden insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Mathias Benedek,et al.  Brain mechanisms associated with internally directed attention and self-generated thought , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[49]  Mathias Benedek,et al.  Looking for ideas: Eye behavior during goal-directed internally focused cognition☆ , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[50]  O D Creutzfeldt,et al.  EEG changes during performance of various tasks under open- and closed-eye conditions. , 1969, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  John Kounios,et al.  The Aha! Moment , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[53]  M. Benedek,et al.  Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccadic Efficacy and Contribution to Foveal and Peripheral Vision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  J. Lisman,et al.  Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention , 2016, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[57]  Alan D Baddeley,et al.  Eyeclosure helps memory by reducing cognitive load and enhancing visualisation , 2011, Memory & cognition.