Colour constancy and conscious perception of changes of illuminant

A sudden change in illuminant (e.g., the outcome of turning on a tungsten light in a room illuminated with dim, natural daylight) causes a "global" change in perceived colour which subjects often recognise as a change of illuminant. In spite of this distinct, global change in the perceptual appearance of the scene caused by significant changes in the wavelength composition of the light reflected from different objects under the new illuminant, the perceived colour of the objects remains largely unchanged and this cornerstone property of human vision is often described as instantaneous colour constancy (ICC). ICC mechanisms are often difficult to study. The generation of appropriate stimuli to isolate ICC mechanisms remains a difficult task since the extraction of colour signals is also confounded in the processing of spatial chromatic context that leads to ICC. The extraction of differences in chromaticity that describe spatial changes in the wavelength composition of the light on the retina is a necessary operation that must precede colour constancy computations. A change of illuminant or changes in the spectral reflectance of the elements that make up the scene under a constant illuminant cause spatial changes in chromatic context and are likely to drive colour constancy mechanisms, but not exclusively. The same stimulus changes also cause differences in local luminance contrast and overall light flux changes, stimulus attributes that can activate different areas of the visual cortex. In order to address this problem we carried out a series of dichoptic experiments designed to investigate how the colour signals from the two eyes are combined in dichoptically viewed Mondrians and the extent to which the processing of chromatic context in monocularly driven neurons contributes to ICC. The psychophysical findings show that normal levels of ICC can be achieved in dichoptic experiments, even when the subject remains unaware of any changes of illuminant. Functional MRI (fMRI) experiments using new stimuli that produce stimulation of colour constancy mechanisms only in one condition with little or no difference in the activity generated in colour processing mechanisms in both test and reference conditions were also carried out. The results show that the processing of ICC signals generates strong activation in V1 and the fusiform colour area (V4, V4A). Significant activation was also observed in areas V2 and V3.

[1]  H Spekreijse,et al.  The cone/horizontal cell network: A possible site for color constancy , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Zeki,et al.  The architecture of the colour centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review * , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual Field Maps in Human Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[4]  S. Zeki Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition , 1983, Neuroscience.

[5]  R. Shapley,et al.  Cone inputs in macaque primary visual cortex. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Long-range colour-generating interactions across the retina , 1986, Nature.

[7]  John L Barbur,et al.  'Double-blindsight' revealed through the processing of color and luminance contrast defined motion signals. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[8]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia , 1993, Neurology.

[9]  T. Wachtler,et al.  Modeling color percepts of dichromats , 2004, Vision Research.

[10]  G. Plant,et al.  Insights into the different exploits of colour in the visual cortex , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[12]  E. Land,et al.  Colors seen in a flash of light. , 1962, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  S. McKee,et al.  Quantitative studies in retinex theory a comparison between theoretical predictions and observer responses to the “color mondrian” experiments , 1976, Vision Research.

[14]  A. Cowey,et al.  Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Cortical Color Blindness is Not “Blindsight for Color” , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[16]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Colour identification and colour constancy are impaired in a patient with incomplete achromatopsia associated with prestriate cortical lesions , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  S Zeki,et al.  A psychophysical dissection of the brain sites involved in color-generating comparisons. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  John L. Barbur,et al.  Study of instantaneous color constancy mechanisms in human vision , 2004, J. Electronic Imaging.

[19]  S Zeki,et al.  The clinical and functional measurement of cortical (in)activity in the visual brain, with special reference to the two subdivisions (V4 and V4 alpha) of the human colour centre. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  E. Land Recent advances in retinex theory , 1986, Vision Research.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The colour centre in the cerebral cortex of man , 1989, Nature.

[23]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[24]  C. Koch,et al.  Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? , 1995, Nature.

[25]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Form and motion from colour in cerebral achromatopsia , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  L M Vaina,et al.  Functional segregation of color and motion processing in the human visual cortex: clinical evidence. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published December 28, 2005 Color Architecture in Alert Macaque Cortex , 2022 .

[28]  David H Foster,et al.  Detecting changes of spatial cone-excitation ratios in dichoptic viewing , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  Gunther Wyszecki,et al.  Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition , 2000 .

[30]  Anya Hurlbert,et al.  Colour Vision: Primary Visual Cortex Shows Its Influence , 2003, Current Biology.

[31]  S. Zeki,et al.  A century of cerebral achromatopsia. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  A. Cowey,et al.  Detection and discrimination of chromatic targets in hemianopic macaque monkeys and humans , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[33]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  The unseen color aftereffect of an unseen stimulus: insight from blindsight into mechanisms of color afterimages. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  M. H. Brill,et al.  Necessary and sufficient conditions for Von Kries chromatic adaptation to give color constancy , 1982, Journal of mathematical biology.

[35]  David H. Foster,et al.  An operational approach to colour constancy , 1992, Vision Research.

[36]  Bevil R Conway,et al.  A different point of hue. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Primate cortical area V4 important for colour constancy but not wavelength discrimination , 1985, Nature.

[38]  V. Walsh,et al.  How does the cortex construct color? , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Steven K Shevell,et al.  A central mechanism of chromatic contrast , 2000, Vision Research.