Sense of control depends on fluency of action selection, not motor performance
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Martin Eimer,et al. Active masks and active inhibition: a comment on Lleras and Enns (2004) and on Verleger, Jaskowski, Aydemir, van der Lubbe, and Groen (2004). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[2] P. Haggard,et al. Awareness of action: Inference and prediction , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[3] H. Bowman,et al. Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming , 2006 .
[4] Marc Jeannerod,et al. Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study , 2003, NeuroImage.
[5] A. Newen,et al. Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[6] Atsushi Sato,et al. Illusion of sense of self-agency: discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership , 2005, Cognition.
[7] Martin Eimer,et al. Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] D. Wegner,et al. Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. , 1999, The American psychologist.
[9] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Limits on Introspection Distorted Subjective Time During the Dual-Task Bottleneck , 2008 .
[10] Brian W. Haas,et al. Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited to concrete stimulus-response associations. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] Saul Sternberg,et al. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .
[12] M. Eimer,et al. Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[13] D. M. Green,et al. Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .
[14] J. Schwarzbach,et al. Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[15] P. Haggard,et al. Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.
[16] Asher Koriat,et al. The Feeling of Knowing: Some Metatheoretical Implications for Consciousness and Control , 2000, Consciousness and Cognition.
[17] Rachel A. Simmons,et al. The negative compatibility effect: A case for self-inhibition , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[18] B. Sparrow,et al. Vicarious agency: experiencing control over the movements of others. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[19] E. Higgins,et al. Motivation in Mental Accessibility: Relevance Of A Representation (ROAR) as a New Framework. , 2010, Social and personality psychology compass.
[20] S. Dehaene,et al. Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.
[21] David Lagnado,et al. Feelings of control: Contingency determines experience of action , 2009, Cognition.
[22] Elisabeth Patherie,et al. The Content of Intentions , 2000 .
[23] E. Pacherie. The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework , 2008, Cognition.
[24] Thomas Schmidt,et al. Criteria for unconscious cognition: Three types of dissociation , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[25] D. Vorberg,et al. The time course of response inhibition in masked priming , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] Atsushi Sato,et al. Both motor prediction and conceptual congruency between preview and action-effect contribute to explicit judgment of agency , 2009, Cognition.
[27] M Eimer,et al. A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] Thomas Goschke,et al. Unconscious modulation of the conscious experience of voluntary control , 2007, Cognition.
[29] C. Frith,et al. Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.
[30] Peter Carruthers,et al. The illusion of conscious will , 2007, Synthese.
[31] Piotr Jaśkowski,et al. The negative compatibility effect with nonmasking flankers: A case for mask-triggered inhibition hypothesis , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[32] W. Prinz,et al. An online neural substrate for sense of agency. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.
[33] Santiago Arango-Munoz. Two Levels of Metacognition , 2011 .
[34] M. Erdelyi,et al. Subliminal perception and its cognates: Theory, indeterminacy, and time , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.
[35] D. Wolpert,et al. Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[36] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Mapping introspection’s blind spot: Reconstruction of dual-task phenomenology using quantified introspection , 2010, Cognition.
[37] M. Husain,et al. At the Edge of Consciousness : Automatic Motor Activation and Voluntary Control , 2008 .
[38] D. Wolpert,et al. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.
[39] M. Hallett,et al. The neural processes underlying self-agency. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.
[40] E. Koechlin,et al. What Are They Up To? The Role of Sensory Evidence and Prior Knowledge in Action Understanding , 2011, PloS one.
[41] D. Wolpert,et al. Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[42] P. Haggard,et al. Voluntary action and conscious awareness , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] David J. Turk,et al. The angular gyrus computes action awareness representations. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[44] S. Sternberg. The discovery of processing stages , 1969 .
[45] M. Eimer,et al. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] Janet Metcalfe,et al. Dissociating Neural Correlates of Action Monitoring and Metacognition of Agency , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[47] Paul Cisek,et al. Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.