The relationship between individual variation in macroscale functional gradients and distinct aspects of ongoing thought

Contemporary accounts of ongoing thought recognise it as a heterogeneous and multidimensional construct, varying in both form and content. An emerging body of evidence demonstrates that distinct types of experience are associated with unique neurocognitive profiles, that can be described at the whole-brain level as interactions between multiple large-scale networks. The current study sought to explore the possibility that whole-brain functional connectivity patterns at rest may be meaningfully related to patterns of ongoing thought that occurred over this period. Participants underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) followed by a questionnaire retrospectively assessing the content and form of their ongoing thoughts during the scan. A non-linear dimension reduction algorithm was applied to the rs-fMRI data to identify components explaining the greatest variance in whole-brain connectivity patterns. Using these data, we examined whether specific types of thought measured at the end of the scan were predictive of individual variation along the first three low-dimensional components of functional connectivity at rest. Multivariate analyses revealed that individuals for whom the connectivity of the sensorimotor system was maximally distinct from the visual system were most likely to report thoughts related to finding solutions to problems or goals and least likely to report thoughts related to the past. These results add to an emerging literature that suggests that unique patterns of experience are associated with distinct distributed neurocognitive profiles and highlight that unimodal systems may play an important role in this process.

[1]  Susan L. Whitfield-Gabrieli,et al.  Conn: A Functional Connectivity Toolbox for Correlated and Anticorrelated Brain Networks , 2012, Brain Connect..

[2]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Discovering dynamic brain networks from big data in rest and task , 2017, NeuroImage.

[3]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  BrainSpace: a toolbox for the analysis of macroscale gradients in neuroimaging and connectomics datasets , 2019, Communications Biology.

[4]  T. Rogers,et al.  The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Kane,et al.  Working Memory Capacity, Mind Wandering, and Creative Cognition: An Individual-Differences Investigation into the Benefits of Controlled Versus Spontaneous Thought. , 2016, Psychology of aesthetics, creativity, and the arts.

[6]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Modes of operation: A topographic neural gradient supporting stimulus dependent and independent cognition , 2019, NeuroImage.

[7]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Dynamic functional connectivity: Promise, issues, and interpretations , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Cross-species functional alignment reveals evolutionary hierarchy within the connectome , 2020, NeuroImage.

[9]  Kevin C. Japardi,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of divergent and convergent thinking in Big-C creativity , 2018, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Boris C. Bernhardt,et al.  Gradients of structure–function tethering across neocortex , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Michael P. Milham,et al.  Toward a connectivity gradient-based framework for reproducible biomarker discovery , 2020, NeuroImage.

[12]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Facing up to the wandering mind: Patterns of off-task laboratory thought are associated with stronger neural recruitment of right fusiform cortex while processing facial stimuli , 2020, NeuroImage.

[13]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  A Correspondence between Individual Differences in the Brain's Intrinsic Functional Architecture and the Content and Form of Self-Generated Thoughts , 2014, PloS one.

[14]  P. Totterdell,et al.  Love is the triumph of the imagination: Daydreams about significant others are associated with increased happiness, love and connection , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[15]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Toward a connectivity gradient-based framework for reproducible biomarker discovery , 2020, NeuroImage.

[16]  Timothy O. Laumann,et al.  Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI , 2014, NeuroImage.

[17]  G. Winocur,et al.  Episodic Memory and Beyond: The Hippocampus and Neocortex in Transformation. , 2016, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  Peter Totterdell,et al.  Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.

[19]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Emergence of neural dynamics within a co-ordinate space of large-scale neural hierarchies , 2019 .

[20]  D. Margulies,et al.  Neurocognitive patterns dissociating semantic processing from executive control are linked to more detailed off-task mental time travel , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[21]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Distant from input: Evidence of regions within the default mode network supporting perceptually-decoupled and conceptually-guided cognition , 2018, NeuroImage.

[22]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Dimensions of Experience: Exploring the Heterogeneity of the Wandering Mind , 2018, Psychological science.

[23]  J. Smallwood,et al.  Inspired by Distraction , 2012, Psychological science.

[24]  Thomas T. Liu,et al.  A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI , 2007, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Vogelstein,et al.  Cross-species functional alignment reveals evolutionary hierarchy within the connectome , 2019, NeuroImage.

[26]  Reinder Vos de Wael,et al.  Anatomical and microstructural determinants of hippocampal subfield functional connectome embedding , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  J. Duncan The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  D. Schacter,et al.  Mind-Wandering as a Natural Kind: A Family-Resemblances View , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Tracking thoughts: Exploring the neural architecture of mental time travel during mind-wandering , 2017, NeuroImage.

[30]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  D. Margulies,et al.  Reductions in task positive neural systems occur with the passage of time and are associated with changes in ongoing thought , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[32]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Aaron Kucyi,et al.  Just a thought: How mind-wandering is represented in dynamic brain connectivity , 2017, NeuroImage.

[34]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  How do we decide what to do? Resting-state connectivity patterns and components of self-generated thought linked to the development of more concrete personal goals , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  F. Tong,et al.  Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas , 2009, Nature.

[37]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Is self-generated thought a means of social problem solving? , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[38]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  The neurocognitive basis of knowledge about object identity and events: dissociations reflect opposing effects of semantic coherence and control , 2019, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[39]  Lars Muckli,et al.  What are we missing here? Brain imaging evidence for higher cognitive functions in primary visual cortex V1 , 2010 .

[40]  Reinder Vos de Wael,et al.  Atypical functional connectome hierarchy in autism , 2018, Nature Communications.

[41]  D. Margulies,et al.  Distinct patterns of thought mediate the link between brain functional connectomes and well-being , 2020, Network Neuroscience.

[42]  D. Gilbert,et al.  A Wandering Mind Is an Unhappy Mind , 2010, Science.

[43]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Individual variation in the propensity for prospective thought is associated with functional integration between visual and retrosplenial cortex , 2018, Cortex.

[44]  Yoed N. Kenett,et al.  The role of the motor system in generating creative thoughts , 2020, NeuroImage.

[45]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Jonathan W. Schooler,et al.  When the Muses Strike: Creative Ideas of Physicists and Writers Routinely Occur During Mind Wandering , 2019, Psychological science.

[47]  Lars Muckli,et al.  What are we missing here? Brain imaging evidence for higher cognitive functions in primary visual cortex V1 , 2010, Int. J. Imaging Syst. Technol..

[48]  Danielle S. Bassett,et al.  Multi-scale brain networks , 2016, NeuroImage.

[49]  D. Margulies,et al.  Default mode network can support the level of detail in experience during active task states , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Evan M. Gordon,et al.  Local-Global Parcellation of the Human Cerebral Cortex From Intrinsic Functional Connectivity MRI , 2017, bioRxiv.

[51]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Going AWOL in the Brain: Mind Wandering Reduces Cortical Analysis of External Events , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[53]  Reinder Vos de Wael,et al.  Shifts in myeloarchitecture characterise adolescent development of cortical gradients , 2019, eLife.

[54]  Boris C. Bernhardt,et al.  BrainSpace: a toolbox for the analysis of macroscale gradients in neuroimaging and connectomics datasets , 2020, Communications Biology.

[55]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Pupillometric Evidence for the Decoupling of Attention from Perceptual Input during Offline Thought , 2011, PloS one.

[56]  Hao-Ting Wang,et al.  Representing Representation: Integration between the Temporal Lobe and the Posterior Cingulate Influences the Content and Form of Spontaneous Thought , 2016, PloS one.