Perception and reproduction of force direction in the horizontal plane.

In this study, we evaluated the capacity of human beings to perceive and reproduce forces applied to the hand. We tested for perceptive distortions and/or privileged directions in the performance of these two tasks. Subjects resisted a reference force applied by a joystick in a given direction, with instructions to keep the hand at a constant position. In a perception task, subjects subsequently resisted a second such force, the direction of which they could adjust with a potentiometer; the task was to reorient the second force to be in the same perceived direction as the reference. In a reproduction task, subjects were instructed to push against the now elastically constrained joystick with the same force that was required to resist the initially applied reference force. Twenty-four reference force directions in the horizontal plane were tested twice each. We observed systematic distortions in the reproduction of force direction that were not present in the perception task. We further observed that the distortions could be predicted by anisotropy of limb stiffness and could be affected by manipulating the mechanical impedance of the hand-joystick interaction. We conclude that human subjects specify and store forces to be applied by the hand not in terms of a perceived force vector, but rather in terms of the motor activity required to resist or produce the force-i.e., subjects possess a multi-dimensional "sense of effort."

[1]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Human arm stiffness and equilibrium-point trajectory during multi-joint movement , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[3]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Equilibrium-Point Control Hypothesis Examined by Measured Arm Stiffness During Multijoint Movement , 1996, Science.

[4]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptic Perception of Spatial Relations , 1999, Perception.

[5]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Transformation from Head- to Shoulder-Centered Representation of Target Direction in Arm Movements , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  E. Gentaz,et al.  The Haptic ‘Oblique Effect’ in Children's and Adults' Perception of Orientation , 1995, Perception.

[7]  E C Lechelt,et al.  Spatial Anisotropy in Intramodal and Cross-Modal Judgments of Stimulus Orientation: The Stability of the Oblique Effect , 1980, Perception.

[8]  S Appelle,et al.  Effect of Modality-Specific Experience on Visual and Haptic Judgment of Orientation , 1985, Perception.

[9]  Effect of muscle tendon vibration on the perception of force , 1985, Experimental Neurology.

[10]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Mechanical properties of muscles: Implications for motor control , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  M. Swash,et al.  The Motor Cortex , 1990 .

[12]  E. Lechelt,et al.  Perceptual orientational asymmetries: A comparison of visual and haptic space , 1976 .

[13]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  T. Flash,et al.  The control of hand equilibrium trajectories in multi-joint arm movements , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[15]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  P R Burgess,et al.  The sense of effort and two models of single-joint motor control. , 1995, Somatosensory & motor research.

[17]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  J. McIntyre,et al.  Servo Hypotheses for the Biological Control of Movement. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  M I Lipshits,et al.  Measurements of human force control during a constrained arm motion using a force-actuated joystick. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Lacquaniti,et al.  Visuo‐motor transformations for arm reaching , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Large Systematic Deviations in the Haptic Perception of Parallelity , 1999, Perception.

[22]  Lynette A. Jones,et al.  Force sensation in isometric contractions: a relative force effect , 1982, Brain Research.

[23]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Analysis of Pointing Errors Reveals Properties of Data Representations and Coordinate Transformations Within the Central Nervous System , 2000, Neural Computation.

[24]  L. Jami Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. , 1992, Physiological reviews.

[25]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Systematic changes in motor cortex cell activity with arm posture during directional isometric force generation. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  A. G. Feldman Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model) for motor control. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  L. Jones,et al.  Perception of force and weight: theory and research. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered and body-centered frames of reference in direct visuomotor transformations , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  The motor cortex and the coding of force. , 1992, Science.

[31]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Sense of muscular effort and somesthetic afferent information in humans. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[32]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  On the relations between single cell activity in the motor cortex and the direction and magnitude of three-dimensional static isometric force , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  F. Lacquaniti Central representations of human limb movement as revealed by studies of drawing and handwriting , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Fel'dman Ag On functional tuning of nervous system during controlled or preservation of stationary pose. 3. Mechanographic analysis of human performance of simple movement tasks , 1966 .

[36]  B. Bigland-ritchie,et al.  Sensation of static force in muscles of different length , 1979, Experimental Neurology.

[37]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a "sense of effort". , 1974, Experimental neurology.

[39]  I. W. Hunter,et al.  Effect of fatigue on force sensation , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[40]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[41]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  G. S. Watson,et al.  ON THE CONSTRUCTION OF SIGNIFICANCE TESTS ON THE CIRCLE AND THE SPHERE , 1956 .

[43]  N. Hogan An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.