The Origin of Behavioral Bursts in Decision-Making Circuitry

From ants to humans, the timing of many animal behaviors comes in bursts of activity separated by long periods of inactivity. Recently, mathematical modeling has shown that simple algorithms of priority-driven behavioral choice can result in bursty behavior. To experimentally test this link between decision-making circuitry and bursty dynamics, we have turned to Drosophila melanogaster. We have found that the statistics of intervals between activity periods in endogenous activity-rest switches of wild-type Drosophila are very well described by the Weibull distribution, a common distribution of bursty dynamics in complex systems. The bursty dynamics of wild-type Drosophila walking activity are shown to be determined by this inter-event distribution alone and not by memory effects, thus resembling human dynamics. Further, using mutant flies that disrupt dopaminergic signaling or the mushroom body, circuitry implicated in decision-making, we show that the degree of behavioral burstiness can be modified. These results are thus consistent with the proposed link between decision-making circuitry and bursty dynamics, and highlight the importance of using simple experimental systems to test general theoretical models of behavior. The findings further suggest that analysis of bursts could prove useful for the study and evaluation of decision-making circuitry.

[1]  C. Peng,et al.  Mosaic organization of DNA nucleotides. , 1994, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[2]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  An engram found? Evaluating the evidence from fruit flies , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[3]  Feng Yu,et al.  Mushroom bodies modulate salience‐based selective fixation behavior in Drosophila , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  Yoshinori Aso,et al.  The Mushroom Body of Adult Drosophila Characterized by GAL4 Drivers , 2009, Journal of neurogenetics.

[5]  H. Stanley,et al.  Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. , 1995, Chaos.

[6]  Yueqing Peng,et al.  Dopamine-Mushroom Body Circuit Regulates Saliency-Based Decision-Making in Drosophila , 2007, Science.

[7]  Jean-René Martin,et al.  The Power Law Distribution for Walking-Time Intervals Correlates with the Ellipsoid-Body in Drosophila , 2001, Journal of neurogenetics.

[8]  Roland Strauss,et al.  Visual Targeting of Motor Actions in Climbing Drosophila , 2010, Current Biology.

[9]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Frontiers in Neural Circuits Neural Circuits , 2022 .

[10]  P. A. Prince,et al.  Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses , 1996, Nature.

[11]  Mark Buchanan Laws, power laws and statistics , 2008 .

[12]  Aike Guo,et al.  Choice strategies in Drosophila are based on competition between olfactory memories , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  R. Hidalgo,et al.  Conditions for the emergence of scaling in the inter-event time of uncorrelated and seasonal systems , 2006 .

[14]  H. Stanley,et al.  Common scale-invariant patterns of sleep-wake transitions across mammalian species. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  J. Armstrong,et al.  Genetic analysis of the Drosophila ellipsoid body neuropil: organization and development of the central complex. , 1999, Journal of neurobiology.

[16]  T. Kitamoto Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. , 2001, Journal of neurobiology.

[17]  Jay Hirsh,et al.  Targeted gene expression in Drosophila dopaminergic cells using regulatory sequences from tyrosine hydroxylase. , 2003, Journal of neurobiology.

[18]  Kei Ito,et al.  Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom body , 2008, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[19]  A Guo,et al.  Choice Behavior of Drosophila Facing Contradictory Visual Cues , 2001, Science.

[20]  Allan I Pack,et al.  Rest in Drosophila Is a Sleep-like State , 2000, Neuron.

[21]  Jean-René Martin A portrait of locomotor behaviour in Drosophila determined by a video-tracking paradigm , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[22]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Flexibility in a single behavioral variable of Drosophila. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[23]  A. M. Edwards,et al.  Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer , 2007, Nature.

[24]  Nicolas E. Humphries,et al.  Scaling laws of marine predator search behaviour , 2008, Nature.

[25]  A. Kanellopoulos,et al.  Protection from premature habituation requires functional mushroom bodies in Drosophila. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[26]  N. Perrimon,et al.  Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. , 1993, Development.

[27]  Adilson E. Motter,et al.  A Poissonian explanation for heavy tails in e-mail communication , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  B. Cole Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of Drosophila , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[29]  Kwang-Il Goh,et al.  Burstiness and memory in complex systems , 2006 .

[30]  Albert-László Barabási,et al.  The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics , 2005, Nature.

[31]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Central complex substructures are required for the maintenance of locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[32]  Zhefeng Gong,et al.  Differential roles of the fan-shaped body and the ellipsoid body in Drosophila visual pattern memory. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[33]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  An engram found? Evaluating the evidence from fruit flies [Curr. Opin. Neuro. 14 (2004) 737] , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  Tim Tully,et al.  Associative Learning Disrupted by Impaired Gs Signaling in Drosophila Mushroom Bodies , 1996, Science.

[35]  B. Brembs,et al.  Order in Spontaneous Behavior , 2007, PloS one.

[36]  Aike Guo,et al.  Increased dopaminergic signaling impairs aversive olfactory memory retention in Drosophila. , 2008, Biochemical and biophysical research communications.

[37]  Shamik Dasgupta,et al.  A Neural Circuit Mechanism Integrating Motivational State with Memory Expression in Drosophila , 2009, Cell.

[38]  John A. Wiens,et al.  Fractal patterns of insect movement in microlandscape mosaics , 1995 .

[39]  Albert-László Barabási,et al.  Modeling bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics , 2005, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[40]  David T. Suzuki,et al.  Temperature-sensitive mutations in Drosophila melanogaster , 1970, Molecular and General Genetics MGG.

[41]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Mushroom bodies suppress locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[42]  P. Greengard,et al.  Writing Memories with Light-Addressable Reinforcement Circuitry , 2009, Cell.

[43]  S. Waddell,et al.  Sequential Use of Mushroom Body Neuron Subsets during Drosophila Odor Memory Processing , 2007, Neuron.

[44]  B. Brembs Mushroom Bodies Regulate Habit Formation in Drosophila , 2009, Current Biology.

[45]  R. Strauss,et al.  Behavioral consequences of dopamine deficiency in the Drosophila central nervous system , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Andreas Voss,et al.  Can a fly ride a bicycle , 1992 .

[47]  M. Koganezawa,et al.  Memory effects on scale-free dynamics in foraging Drosophila. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[48]  Sang Ki Park,et al.  Dopamine Is a Regulator of Arousal in the Fruit Fly , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  L. Amaral,et al.  On Universality in Human Correspondence Activity , 2009, Science.

[50]  R. Dean Malmgren,et al.  Comment on Barabasi, Nature 435, 207 (2005) , 2005 .

[51]  A. Reynolds,et al.  Free-Flight Odor Tracking in Drosophila Is Consistent with an Optimal Intermittent Scale-Free Search , 2007, PloS one.

[52]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Temporal pattern of locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.