Temporal expectancies driven by self- and externally generated rhythms

&NA; The dynamic attending theory proposes that rhythms entrain periodic fluctuations of attention which modulate the gain of sensory input. However, temporal expectancies can also be driven by the mere passage of time (foreperiod effect). It is currently unknown how these two types of temporal expectancy relate to each other, i.e. whether they work in parallel and have distinguishable neural signatures. The current research addresses this issue. Participants either tapped a 1 Hz rhythm (active task) or were passively presented with the same rhythm using tactile stimulators (passive task). Based on this rhythm an auditory target was then presented early, in synchrony, or late. Behavioural results were in line with the dynamic attending theory as RTs were faster for in‐ compared to out‐of‐synchrony targets. Electrophysiological results suggested self‐generated and externally induced rhythms to entrain neural oscillations in the delta frequency band. Auditory ERPs showed evidence of two distinct temporal expectancy processes. Both tasks demonstrated a pattern which followed a linear foreperiod effect. In the active task, however, we also observed an ERP effect consistent with the dynamic attending theory. This study shows that temporal expectancies generated by a rhythm and expectancy generated by the mere passage of time can work in parallel and sheds light on how these mechanisms are implemented in the brain. HighlightsTemporal expectancies can be driven by rhythms or by the mere passage of time.We conceived a paradigm using EEG that tests for both these forms of expectancy.We compared self‐generated and externally generated rhythms.We found evidence for both expectancy processes working in parallel.

[1]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Finding and Feeling the Musical Beat: Striatal Dissociations between Detection and Prediction of Regularity , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Alexander Jones Independent effects of bottom-up temporal expectancy and top-down spatial attention. An audiovisual study using rhythmic cueing , 2015, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[3]  T. Hackett,et al.  Predictive motor control of sensory dynamics in auditory active sensing , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  Nicol S Harper,et al.  Temporal predictability enhances auditory detection. , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  K. Crowley,et al.  A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  Daniel Sanabria,et al.  Rhythms that speed you up. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  C. Ponton,et al.  Central Auditory Plasticity: Changes in the N1-P2 Complex after Speech-Sound Training , 2001, Ear and hearing.

[9]  Lisa D. Sanders,et al.  Musical Meter Modulates the Allocation of Attention across Time , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Mari Riess Jones,et al.  Attending to sound patterns and the role of entrainment , 2010 .

[11]  M. Jones,et al.  Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  Ramesh Balasubramaniam,et al.  Physical and neural entrainment to rhythm: human sensorimotor coordination across tasks and effector systems , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[13]  D. Woods The component structure of the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked potential. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[14]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Neuronal Oscillations and Multisensory Interaction in Primary Auditory Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[15]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Daniel Sanabria,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence of temporal preparation driven by rhythms in audition , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[17]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Delta-Beta Coupled Oscillations Underlie Temporal Prediction Accuracy. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Orienting Attention to Points in Time Improves Stimulus Processing Both within and across Modalities , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. , 1976, Psychological review.

[20]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distribution of human auditory evoked potentials. II. Evidence for overlapping sources and involvement of auditory cortex. , 1982, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Synergistic Effect of Combined Temporal and Spatial Expectations on Visual Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Action enhances auditory but not visual temporal sensitivity , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  S. K. Barber,et al.  Knowledge of Stimulus Repetition Affects the Magnitude and Spatial Distribution of Low-Frequency Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2002, Audiology and Neurotology.

[24]  Jessica M. Ross,et al.  Motor simulation theories of musical beat perception , 2016, Neurocase.

[25]  R. Brochard,et al.  Got rhythm… for better and for worse. Cross-modal effects of auditory rhythm on visual word recognition , 2013, Cognition.

[26]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Carmen C Canavier,et al.  Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[29]  Björn Herrmann,et al.  Low-Frequency Neural Oscillations Support Dynamic Attending in Temporal Context , 2014 .

[30]  Christoph S. Herrmann,et al.  Auditory rhythms entrain visual processes in the human brain: Evidence from evoked oscillations and event-related potentials , 2015, NeuroImage.

[31]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Attending points in time and space , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. E. Schulman,et al.  Functional coupling and regional activation of human cortical motor areas during simple, internally paced and externally paced finger movements. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  Michael Schutz,et al.  Movement Enhances Perceived Timing in the Absence of Auditory Feedback , 2015 .

[34]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  R. VanRullen Perceptual Cycles , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  David Poeppel,et al.  Rhythmicity and cross-modal temporal cues facilitate detection , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  A. Nobre,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Related to Anticipatory Attention Follow Temporal Expectations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Saskia Haegens,et al.  Temporal expectations and neural amplitude fluctuations in auditory cortex interactively influence perception , 2016, NeuroImage.

[39]  E. Large,et al.  The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .

[40]  César F. Lima,et al.  Roles of Supplementary Motor Areas in Auditory Processing and Auditory Imagery , 2016, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  G. Karmos,et al.  Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection , 2008, Science.

[42]  Ankoor S. Shah,et al.  An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Robert Egly,et al.  Chronometric evidence for entrained attention , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  M. Jones,et al.  Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.

[46]  Charles E. Schroeder,et al.  Motor contributions to the temporal precision of auditory attention , 2014, Nature Communications.

[47]  Anna-Katharina R. Bauer,et al.  The auditory dynamic attending theory revisited: A closer look at the pitch comparison task , 2015, Brain Research.

[48]  Stéphane Perrey,et al.  Expectations induced by natural-like temporal fluctuations are independent of attention decrement: Evidence from behavior and early visual evoked potentials , 2015, NeuroImage.

[49]  Julian Keil,et al.  Temporal orienting precedes intersensory attention and has opposing effects on early evoked brain activity , 2017, NeuroImage.

[50]  A. Nobre,et al.  Temporal Expectation Improves the Quality of Sensory Information , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  A. Nobre,et al.  Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[53]  Guy Dove,et al.  Linking Brainwaves to the Brain: An ERP Primer , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[54]  Deborah J Serrien,et al.  The neural dynamics of timed motor tasks: evidence from a synchronization–continuation paradigm , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[55]  F. Castellanos,et al.  Entrainment of neural oscillations as a modifiable substrate of attention , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Kathrin Lange,et al.  The N1 effect of temporal attention is independent of sound location and intensity: implications for possible mechanisms of temporal attention. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[57]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  Annett Schirmer,et al.  Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[59]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.

[60]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Neuronal oscillations as a mechanistic substrate of auditory temporal prediction , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[61]  Bettina Forster,et al.  Neural correlates of endogenous attention, exogenous attention and inhibition of return in touch , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[62]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Motor Intention Determines Sensory Attenuation of Brain Responses to Self-initiated Sounds , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[64]  Assaf Breska,et al.  Automatic Bias of Temporal Expectations following Temporally Regular Input Independently of High-level Temporal Expectation , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  A. Nobre,et al.  Time for the Fourth Dimension in Attention , 2014 .

[66]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception , 2010 .

[67]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[68]  A. Amochaev,et al.  Knowledge of stimulus timing attenuates human evoked cortical potentials. , 1981, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[69]  Pamela Baess,et al.  Action–Sound Coincidences Suppress Evoked Responses of the Human Auditory Cortex in EEG and MEG , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[71]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[72]  E. Schafer,et al.  Self-Stimulation Alters Human Sensory Brain Responses , 1973, Science.

[73]  Lisa D. Sanders,et al.  Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: Event-related potential evidence , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[74]  K. Lange The ups and downs of temporal orienting: a review of auditory temporal orienting studies and a model associating the heterogeneous findings on the auditory N1 with opposite effects of attention and prediction , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[75]  Joachim Gross,et al.  Auditory cortical delta-entrainment interacts with oscillatory power in multiple fronto-parietal networks , 2017, NeuroImage.

[76]  J. Obleser,et al.  Frequency modulation entrains slow neural oscillations and optimizes human listening behavior , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[77]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. , 2002 .

[78]  Kathrin Lange,et al.  Challenging perceptual tasks require more attention: The influence of task difficulty on the N1 effect of temporal orienting , 2014, Brain and Cognition.

[79]  Isabelle Peretz,et al.  Capturing with EEG the neural entrainment and coupling underlying sensorimotor synchronization to the beat. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[80]  D. Schön,et al.  Rhythm implicitly affects temporal orienting of attention across modalities. , 2013, Acta psychologica.