Temporal expectancies driven by self- and externally generated rhythms
暂无分享,去创建一个
Alexander Jones | Lionel Granjon | Florian Waszak | Yi-Fang Hsu | F. Waszak | L. Granjon | Alexander Jones | Y. Hsu | Florian Waszak
[1] Jessica A. Grahn,et al. Finding and Feeling the Musical Beat: Striatal Dissociations between Detection and Prediction of Regularity , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[2] Alexander Jones. Independent effects of bottom-up temporal expectancy and top-down spatial attention. An audiovisual study using rhythmic cueing , 2015, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[3] T. Hackett,et al. Predictive motor control of sensory dynamics in auditory active sensing , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[4] Nicol S Harper,et al. Temporal predictability enhances auditory detection. , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[5] K. Crowley,et al. A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[6] Daniel Sanabria,et al. Rhythms that speed you up. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] A. Nobre,et al. Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[8] C. Ponton,et al. Central Auditory Plasticity: Changes in the N1-P2 Complex after Speech-Sound Training , 2001, Ear and hearing.
[9] Lisa D. Sanders,et al. Musical Meter Modulates the Allocation of Attention across Time , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[10] Mari Riess Jones,et al. Attending to sound patterns and the role of entrainment , 2010 .
[11] M. Jones,et al. Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.
[12] Ramesh Balasubramaniam,et al. Physical and neural entrainment to rhythm: human sensorimotor coordination across tasks and effector systems , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[13] D. Woods. The component structure of the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked potential. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[14] C. Schroeder,et al. Neuronal Oscillations and Multisensory Interaction in Primary Auditory Cortex , 2007, Neuron.
[15] Luc H. Arnal,et al. Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] Daniel Sanabria,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of temporal preparation driven by rhythms in audition , 2013, Biological Psychology.
[17] Luc H. Arnal,et al. Delta-Beta Coupled Oscillations Underlie Temporal Prediction Accuracy. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.
[18] Brigitte Röder,et al. Orienting Attention to Points in Time Improves Stimulus Processing Both within and across Modalities , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[19] M. R. Jones,et al. Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. , 1976, Psychological review.
[20] C. C. Wood,et al. Scalp distribution of human auditory evoked potentials. II. Evidence for overlapping sources and involvement of auditory cortex. , 1982, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[21] Anna C. Nobre,et al. Synergistic Effect of Combined Temporal and Spatial Expectations on Visual Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[22] Marcia Grabowecky,et al. Action enhances auditory but not visual temporal sensitivity , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[23] S. K. Barber,et al. Knowledge of Stimulus Repetition Affects the Magnitude and Spatial Distribution of Low-Frequency Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2002, Audiology and Neurotology.
[24] Jessica M. Ross,et al. Motor simulation theories of musical beat perception , 2016, Neurocase.
[25] R. Brochard,et al. Got rhythm… for better and for worse. Cross-modal effects of auditory rhythm on visual word recognition , 2013, Cognition.
[26] C. Summerfield,et al. Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] Carmen C Canavier,et al. Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[28] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[29] Björn Herrmann,et al. Low-Frequency Neural Oscillations Support Dynamic Attending in Temporal Context , 2014 .
[30] Christoph S. Herrmann,et al. Auditory rhythms entrain visual processes in the human brain: Evidence from evoked oscillations and event-related potentials , 2015, NeuroImage.
[31] Brigitte Röder,et al. Attending points in time and space , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] A. E. Schulman,et al. Functional coupling and regional activation of human cortical motor areas during simple, internally paced and externally paced finger movements. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[33] Michael Schutz,et al. Movement Enhances Perceived Timing in the Absence of Auditory Feedback , 2015 .
[34] Catalin V. Buhusi,et al. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[35] R. VanRullen. Perceptual Cycles , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] David Poeppel,et al. Rhythmicity and cross-modal temporal cues facilitate detection , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[37] A. Nobre,et al. Alpha Oscillations Related to Anticipatory Attention Follow Temporal Expectations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[38] Saskia Haegens,et al. Temporal expectations and neural amplitude fluctuations in auditory cortex interactively influence perception , 2016, NeuroImage.
[39] E. Large,et al. The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .
[40] César F. Lima,et al. Roles of Supplementary Motor Areas in Auditory Processing and Auditory Imagery , 2016, Trends in Neurosciences.
[41] G. Karmos,et al. Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection , 2008, Science.
[42] Ankoor S. Shah,et al. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] R. Schubotz. Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] Robert Egly,et al. Chronometric evidence for entrained attention , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[45] M. Jones,et al. Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.
[46] Charles E. Schroeder,et al. Motor contributions to the temporal precision of auditory attention , 2014, Nature Communications.
[47] Anna-Katharina R. Bauer,et al. The auditory dynamic attending theory revisited: A closer look at the pitch comparison task , 2015, Brain Research.
[48] Stéphane Perrey,et al. Expectations induced by natural-like temporal fluctuations are independent of attention decrement: Evidence from behavior and early visual evoked potentials , 2015, NeuroImage.
[49] Julian Keil,et al. Temporal orienting precedes intersensory attention and has opposing effects on early evoked brain activity , 2017, NeuroImage.
[50] A. Nobre,et al. Temporal Expectation Improves the Quality of Sensory Information , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[51] A. Nobre,et al. Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[52] A. Nobre,et al. The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[53] Guy Dove,et al. Linking Brainwaves to the Brain: An ERP Primer , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.
[54] Deborah J Serrien,et al. The neural dynamics of timed motor tasks: evidence from a synchronization–continuation paradigm , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[55] F. Castellanos,et al. Entrainment of neural oscillations as a modifiable substrate of attention , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[56] Kathrin Lange,et al. The N1 effect of temporal attention is independent of sound location and intensity: implications for possible mechanisms of temporal attention. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[57] M. Shadlen,et al. A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[58] Annett Schirmer,et al. Unattended musical beats enhance visual processing. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[59] C. Gallistel,et al. Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.
[60] C. Schroeder,et al. Neuronal oscillations as a mechanistic substrate of auditory temporal prediction , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[61] Bettina Forster,et al. Neural correlates of endogenous attention, exogenous attention and inhibition of return in touch , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.
[62] Erich Schröger,et al. Motor Intention Determines Sensory Attenuation of Brain Responses to Self-initiated Sounds , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[63] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .
[64] Assaf Breska,et al. Automatic Bias of Temporal Expectations following Temporally Regular Input Independently of High-level Temporal Expectation , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[65] A. Nobre,et al. Time for the Fourth Dimension in Attention , 2014 .
[66] R. VanRullen,et al. The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception , 2010 .
[67] Anna C Nobre,et al. Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[68] A. Amochaev,et al. Knowledge of stimulus timing attenuates human evoked cortical potentials. , 1981, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[69] Pamela Baess,et al. Action–Sound Coincidences Suppress Evoked Responses of the Human Auditory Cortex in EEG and MEG , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[70] R. Näätänen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[71] C. Schroeder,et al. Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.
[72] E. Schafer,et al. Self-Stimulation Alters Human Sensory Brain Responses , 1973, Science.
[73] Lisa D. Sanders,et al. Temporally selective attention modulates early perceptual processing: Event-related potential evidence , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[74] K. Lange. The ups and downs of temporal orienting: a review of auditory temporal orienting studies and a model associating the heterogeneous findings on the auditory N1 with opposite effects of attention and prediction , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[75] Joachim Gross,et al. Auditory cortical delta-entrainment interacts with oscillatory power in multiple fronto-parietal networks , 2017, NeuroImage.
[76] J. Obleser,et al. Frequency modulation entrains slow neural oscillations and optimizes human listening behavior , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[77] H. Zelaznik,et al. Dissociation of explicit and implicit timing in repetitive tapping and drawing movements. , 2002 .
[78] Kathrin Lange,et al. Challenging perceptual tasks require more attention: The influence of task difficulty on the N1 effect of temporal orienting , 2014, Brain and Cognition.
[79] Isabelle Peretz,et al. Capturing with EEG the neural entrainment and coupling underlying sensorimotor synchronization to the beat. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.
[80] D. Schön,et al. Rhythm implicitly affects temporal orienting of attention across modalities. , 2013, Acta psychologica.