Asking questions can change choice behavior: does it do so automatically or effortfully?

The present research uses a technique that permits unique estimation of both automatic and effortful processes in the question-behavior link. Results show that individuals asked to report behavioral intent (vs. those not asked) are more likely to choose options that are highly accessible and positively valenced, regardless of cognitive resources available at the time of processing. This suggests that the effect of intent questions on subsequent behavior is primarily the result of automatic, as opposed to effortful, processing. Practically, this suggests that efforts to debias this robust effect need to affect nonconscious processes and adjust for the automatic impact of being asked an intention question on respondents' behavior.

[1]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Consequences and causes of correlations in process dissociation. , 1997 .

[2]  C. Judd,et al.  Effects of Repeated Expressions on Attitude Extremity , 1992 .

[3]  P. Graf,et al.  Process dissociation procedure: Handle with caution! , 1994 .

[4]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Implicit memory and test awareness. , 1990 .

[5]  E M Reingold,et al.  Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Vicki G. Morwitz,et al.  Does Measuring Intent Change Behavior , 1993 .

[7]  Gita Venkataramani Johar,et al.  Contingent Processes of Source Identification , 1997 .

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[9]  Martin L. Bink,et al.  Activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions , 1998 .

[10]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Age-related deficits in prospective memory: the influence of task complexity. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[11]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[12]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Separating habit and recollection: memory slips, process dissociations, and probability matching. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  M. Bazerman Judgment in Managerial Decision Making , 1990 .

[14]  G. Fitzsimons,et al.  THE MERE-MEASUREMENT EFFECT : WHY DOES MEASURING PURCHASE INTENTIONS CHANGE ACTUAL PURCHASE BEHAVIOR ? Revised , May 28 , 1999 , 1999 .

[15]  M A Stadler,et al.  Estimating unconscious processes: implications of a general class of models. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming famous without being recognized: Unconscious influences of memory produced by dividing attention , 1989 .

[17]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Retrieval processes in prospective memory : Theoretical approaches and some new empirical findings , 1996 .

[18]  B. Hayes,et al.  Automatic and intentional processes in children's recognition memory: the reversed misinformation effect , 2002 .

[19]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  L. Hasher,et al.  Automatic and effortful processes in memory. , 1979 .

[21]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Neil W. Mulligan,et al.  Measuring the bases of recognition memory: An investigation of the process-dissociation framework , 1997 .

[23]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Measures of Memory , 1988 .

[24]  J. Bargh,et al.  The Role of Consciousness in Priming Effects on Categorization , 1987 .

[25]  S. Sherman On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. , 1980 .

[26]  M. Polanyi,et al.  Personal Knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy , 1959 .

[27]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the process-dissociation procedure? , 1997 .

[28]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[29]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Philip M. Merikle,et al.  Independence or redundancy? Two models of conscious and unconscious influences , 1993 .

[31]  L. Kvavilashvili Remembering intention as a distinct form of memory , 1987 .

[32]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Memory influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. , 1988 .

[33]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[34]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[35]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[36]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. , 1997 .

[37]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. , 1980, Science.

[38]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[39]  D L Hintzman,et al.  Violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Vicki G. Morwitz,et al.  The Effect of Measuring Intent on Brand-Level Purchase Behavior , 1996 .

[41]  T. Scruggs Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities , 1992 .

[42]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .

[43]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Invariance in automatic influences of memory: toward a user's guide for the process-dissociation procedure. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Unconscious Influences of Memory for a Prior Event , 1987 .

[45]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[46]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .