Spatial Representation and Updating: Evidence from Neuropsychological Investigations

How spatial information is represented and updated over time and over changes in the position of the stimulus and/or observer is considered in the context of a population of patients who have impairment in spatial perception. We present data both from our own research with patients suffering from hernispatial neglect as well as from studies in the literature. Taken together, these studies suggest that spatial information is coded in more than one spatial frame of reference simultaneously and that the choice of reference frame depends on the demands of the tasks. Once the stimulus is located, however, the patients are able to update the position of the stimulus dynamically when walking or when undergoing passive rotation. The insights obtained from this neuropsychological population provides converging evidence for the psychological and neural mechanisms which mediate spatial representation and dovetail well with existing single unit recording and functional imaging data.

[1]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Attention to within-object and between-object spatial representations: multiple sites for visual selection , 1994 .

[2]  N. Soroker,et al.  Coordinate Frame for Pattern Recognition in Unilateral Spatial Neglect , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  A. Hillis,et al.  Unilateral spatial neglect in dissociable frames of reference: a comment on Farah, Brunn, Wong, Wallace, and Carpenter (1990) , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  J. Corlett Chapter 16 The Role of Vision in the Planning and Guidance of Locomotion Through the Environment , 1992 .

[5]  T Landis,et al.  To see better to the left when looking more to the right: Effects of gaze direction and frames of spatial coordinates in unilateral neglect , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[6]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Preattentive Processes Guide Visual Search: Evidence from Patients with Unilateral Visual Neglect , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions , 1997 .

[8]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Frames of reference for allocating attention to space: Evidence from the neglect syndrome , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[10]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[11]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Mechanisms Underlying Spatial Representation Revealed through Studies of Hemispatial Neglect , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  The automatic updating of egocentric spatial relationships and its impairment due to right posterior cortical lesions , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  K M Heilman,et al.  A comparison of the influences of body and environment centred reference frames on neglect. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  D. Elliott,et al.  The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  R Caminiti,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Vision and motor control , 1992 .

[18]  W. Heide,et al.  Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[19]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient , 1990, Nature.

[20]  Peter Thier,et al.  Parietal Lobe Contributions to Orientation in 3D Space , 1997 .

[21]  A Berthoz,et al.  Cortical control of vestibular-guided saccades in man. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Neglect dyslexia and the early stages of visual word recognition , 2000 .

[23]  Carl R Olson,et al.  Brain representation of object-centered space , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  M A Goodale,et al.  The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  J. Driver,et al.  Can Visual Neglect Operate in Object-centred Co-ordinates? An Affirmative Single-case Study , 1991 .

[26]  R. Andersen,et al.  Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli , 1995, Nature.

[27]  H. Karnath,et al.  Decrease of contralateral neglect by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-centered not scene-based visual neglect. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Frames of reference in hemineglect: a computational approach. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[30]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Evidence from unilateral visual neglect , 1995 .

[31]  A. Pouget,et al.  Relating unilateral neglect to the neural coding of space , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  “Paradoxical neglect”: spatial representations, hemisphere-specific activation, and spatial cueing , 1995 .

[33]  C R Olson,et al.  Object-centered direction selectivity in the macaque supplementary eye field , 1995, Science.

[34]  H.-O. Karnath,et al.  Neural Encoding of Space Egocentric Coordinates , 1997 .

[35]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992 .

[36]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-based attentional mechanisms: Evidence from patients with unilateral neglect. , 1994 .

[37]  Martin Arguin,et al.  Evidence for an Independent Stimuluscentered Spatial Reference Frame from a Case of Visual Hemineglect , 1993, Cortex.

[38]  Nicola Smania,et al.  Frames of Reference for Mapping Tactile Stimuli in Brain-Damaged Patients , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[40]  K M Heilman,et al.  Hemispatial visual inattention masquerading as hemianopia , 1989, Neurology.

[41]  M. Imbert,et al.  Egocentric reference and asymmetric perception of space , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Jon Driver,et al.  Object-Centered Visual Neglect, or Relative Egocentric Neglect? , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[44]  Carl R Olson,et al.  Object-based vision and attention in primates , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[45]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Object-Centered Neglect in Patients with Unilateral Neglect: Effects of Left-Right Coordinates of Objects , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Spence,et al.  Attention and the crossmodal construction of space , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[49]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  R. Rafal,et al.  Axis-based neglect of visual shapes , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Roberto Cubelli,et al.  Left of what? The role of egocentric coordinates in neglect , 1997, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[52]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Levels of representation, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect , 1990 .

[53]  Serge Brédart,et al.  Semantic memory and amnesia: A case study , 1996 .

[54]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .

[55]  Edoardo Bisiach,et al.  The Spatial Features of Unilateral Neglect , 1997 .

[56]  Matthias Niemeier,et al.  Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behaviour of patients with spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Jon Driver,et al.  Egocentric and object-based visual neglect , 1998 .

[58]  H. Karnath,et al.  Trunk orientation as the determining factor of the 'contralateral' deficit in the neglect syndrome and as the physical anchor of the internal representation of body orientation in space. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[59]  K M Heilman,et al.  Gaze‐dependent hemianopia without hemispatial neglect , 1991, Neurology.

[60]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccadic dysmetria in a patient with a right frontoparietal lesion. The importance of corollary discharge for accurate spatial behaviour. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Stimulus-centered neglect in reading and object recognition , 2000 .

[62]  M Behrmann,et al.  Attention accesses multiple reference frames: evidence from visual neglect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  G. Jones,et al.  Vestibular perceptual deficits in patients with parietal lobe lesions. A preliminary study. , 1991, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[64]  J. Duhamel,et al.  Neglect in Monkeys : effect of permanent and reversible lesions. , 2002 .

[65]  Michael C. Mozer Frames of reference in unilateral neglect and visual perception: a computational perspective. , 2002 .

[66]  A. Young,et al.  Neglect and visual recognition. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[67]  R. Walker,et al.  Object-Based Neglect: An Investigation of the Contributions of Eye Movements and Perceptual Completion , 1996, Cortex.

[68]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Spatial Transformations in the Parietal Cortex Using Basis Functions , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Two basic properties of space representation in the brain: evidence from unilateral neglect. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[70]  J. Walker Saccadic Eye Movements in Object-based Neglect , 1996 .