Temporal horizon: modulation by smoking status and gender.

Recently, delay discounting has been argued to be conceptually consistent with the notion of temporal horizon [Bickel, W.K., Yi, R., Kowal, B.P., Gatchalian, K.M., 2008. Cigarette smokers discount past and future rewards symmetrically and more than controls: is discounting a measure of impulsivity? Drug Alcohol Depend. 96, 256-262]. Temporal horizon refers to the temporal distance over which behavioral events or objects can influence behavior. Here we examine the results on two putative measures of temporal horizon, future time perspective (FTP) and delay discounting, collected over three separate studies (n=227), to determine the influence of smoking and gender on temporal horizon. By comparing the results on these temporal horizon measures we address our population of interest: women who smoke. One of the measures of FTP indicates that smoking women have a shorter temporal horizon than their nonsmoking counterparts. Additionally, the story completion measures of FTP are positively correlated with delay discounting. In contrast, results of delay discounting measures showed no difference between smoking women and nonsmoking women, while results of delay discounting measures indicated smoking men have a shorter temporal horizon than non-smoking men. Additionally, the results of the FTP story completion measure indicated that lower third income earners had a shortened temporal horizon compared to upper third income earners. A possible explanation for these results is explored, and the implications of the modulation of temporal horizon by gender and smoking are discussed.

[1]  M. Kenward,et al.  Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. , 1997, Biometrics.

[2]  Nancy Krieger,et al.  Working class matters: socioeconomic disadvantage, race/ethnicity, gender, and smoking in NHIS 2000. , 2004, American journal of public health.

[3]  Leanne Alvos,et al.  Future time perspective in current and previous injecting drug users. , 1993, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[4]  Daniel D. Holt,et al.  Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[5]  F. Noubary,et al.  Gender differences in depression and smoking among youth in Cape Town, South Africa. , 2006, Ethnicity & disease.

[6]  A. Dewolfe,et al.  Future time perspective in alcoholics, process and reactive schizophrenics, and normals. , 1985, The International journal of the addictions.

[7]  C. Mazure,et al.  Gender differences in the comorbidity of smoking behavior and major depression. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[8]  W. Bickel,et al.  Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[9]  Matthew W. Johnson,et al.  Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  Dallas E. Johnson,et al.  Analysis of messy data , 1992 .

[11]  Taiki Takahashi,et al.  Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[12]  K. Carey,et al.  Associations Among Health Behaviors and Time Perspective in Young Adults: Model Testing with Boot-Strapping Replication , 2006, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[13]  Matthew W. Johnson,et al.  Moderate drug use and delay discounting: a comparison of heavy, light, and never smokers. , 2007, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[14]  L. Epstein,et al.  Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[15]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[16]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[17]  Dallas E. Johnson,et al.  Analysis of Messy Data, Volume III: Analysis of Covariance , 2001 .

[18]  Margo Wilson,et al.  Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  J. Ockene,et al.  Gender, social pressure, and smoking cessations: the Community Intervention Trial for Smoking Cessation (COMMIT) at baseline. , 1997, Social science & medicine.

[20]  J. Richards,et al.  Delay discounting and probability discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[21]  J. Richards,et al.  Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures , 2006 .

[22]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[23]  D. Cross,et al.  Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[24]  B. Reynolds,et al.  Delay discounting by the children of smokers and nonsmokers. , 2009, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[25]  Joy M. Jacobs-Lawson,et al.  Gender Differences in Factors that Influence Time Spent Planning for Retirement , 2004, Journal of women & aging.

[26]  W. Bickel,et al.  Temporal discounting as a measure of executive function: insights from the competing neuro-behavioral decision system hypothesis of addiction. , 2008, Advances in health economics and health services research.

[27]  L. Marks,et al.  Cross-modality matching of brightness and loudness. , 1965, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Amy L Odum,et al.  Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. , 2002, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[29]  W. Bickel,et al.  Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. , 1998, Addiction.

[30]  Warren K Bickel,et al.  Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[31]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  Warren K Bickel,et al.  Cigarette smokers discount past and future rewards symmetrically and more than controls: is discounting a measure of impulsivity? , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[33]  B. Reynolds A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[34]  Kurex Sidik,et al.  Simple heterogeneity variance estimation for meta‐analysis , 2005 .

[35]  P. Zimbardo,et al.  Who's Smoking, Drinking, and Using Drugs? Time Perspective as a Predictor of Substance Use , 1999 .

[36]  R. Wolfinger Covariance structure selection in general mixed models , 1993 .

[37]  M. Carroll,et al.  Biological basis of sex differences in drug abuse: preclinical and clinical studies , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[38]  M WALLACE,et al.  Future time perspective in schizophrenia. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[39]  K. Kirby,et al.  Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  Joel Myerson,et al.  Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Discounting Delayed and Probabilistic Rewards , 2002 .

[41]  K. Koehler Analysis of Messy Data, Vol III: Analysis of Covariance , 2002 .