“Information” and the chick-a-dee call: Communicating with a complex vocal system

[1]  Erick Greene,et al.  Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.

[2]  M. C. Baker,et al.  Black-capped chickadee call dialects along a continuous habitat corridor , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  J. Ekman ECOLOGY OF NON-BREEDING SOCIAL SYSTEMS OF PARUS , 1989 .

[4]  Todd M Freeberg,et al.  Receivers respond differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note composition in Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  E. Wilson,et al.  Sociobiology. The New Synthesis , 1975 .

[6]  J. Hailman,et al.  The ‘chick-a-dee’ calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English , 1985 .

[7]  M. C. Baker,et al.  Population Differentiation in a Complex Bird Sound: A Comparison of Three Bioacoustical Analysis Procedures , 2003 .

[8]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Flock-specific recognition of chickadee calls , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[9]  Christine R. Hurd Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[10]  R. W. Ficken,et al.  Dialects in a call associated with pair interactions in the black-capped chickadee , 1985 .

[11]  R. Weisman,et al.  Open-ended categorization of chick-a-dee calls by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.

[12]  Jack W. Bradbury,et al.  Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .

[13]  Peter Marler,et al.  Bird calls: just emotional displays or something more? , 2008 .

[14]  C. E. Ho,et al.  A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  R. Dawkins,et al.  Animal signals: information or manipulation? , 1978 .

[16]  D. Leger Contextual sources of information and responses to animal communication signals. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al.  Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: The chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) , 2004 .

[18]  F. Sheldon,et al.  PHYLOGENY OF TITMICE (PARIDAE): II. SPECIES RELATIONSHIPS BASED ON SEQUENCES OF THE MITOCHONDRIAL CYTOCHROME-B GENE , 2005 .

[19]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[20]  Jack P. Hailman,et al.  Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘Language’ by structural linguistics , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  The Mathematical Theory of Communication , 1950 .

[22]  Susan T. Smith Communication and other social behavior in Parus carolinensis , 1972 .

[23]  E. Morton,et al.  Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach , 1998 .

[24]  Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al.  Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. II: The chick-a-dee call of the mountain chickadee (Poecile gambeli) , 2004 .

[25]  Jack P. Hailman,et al.  The chick-a-dee call system of the Mexican chickadee , 1994 .

[26]  J. Lucas,et al.  Carolina Chickadee (Aves, Paridae, Poecile carolinensis) Vocalization Rates: Effects of Body Mass and Food Availability under Aviary Conditions , 1999 .

[27]  Robert W. Ficken,et al.  VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEE , 1978 .

[28]  M. Ficken Call Similarities among Mixed Species Flock Associates , 2000 .

[29]  William J. Smith,et al.  The Behavior of Communicating: An Ethological Approach , 1980 .

[30]  April M. Becker,et al.  MOBBING CALLS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES: EFFECTS OF URGENCY ON CALL PRODUCTION , 2002 .

[31]  D. Blumstein,et al.  Does Sociality Drive The Evolution Of Communicative Complexity? A Comparative Test With Ground‐Dwelling Sciurid Alarm Calls , 1997, The American Naturalist.

[32]  Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al.  Chick-a-dee call syntax, social context, and season affect vocal responses of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[33]  Svein Haftorn Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the Willow Tit Parus montanus , 1993 .

[34]  S. Haftorn CONTEXTS AND POSSIBLE FUNCTIONS OF ALARM CALLING IN THE WILLOW TIT, PARUS MONTANUS; THE PRINCIPLE OF ‘BETTER SAFE THAN SORRY’ , 2000 .

[35]  J. Hailman Optical Signals: Animal Communication and Light , 1977 .

[36]  Jack P. Hailman,et al.  Constraints on the Structure of Combinatorial “Chick-a-dee” Calls , 1987 .

[37]  Todd M Freeberg,et al.  Variation in chick-a-dee calls of a Carolina chickadee population, Poecile carolinensis: identity and redundancy within note types. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  R G Weisman,et al.  Call-note discriminations in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[39]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Vocal plasticity in captive black-capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call convergence , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  Dorothy L. Mammen,et al.  Individual differences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[41]  M. Ficken,et al.  A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF PASSERINE MOBBING CALLS , 1996 .

[42]  Philip K Gaddis,et al.  STRUCTURE AND VARIABILITY IN THE VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE MOUNTAIN CHICKADEE , 1985 .

[43]  G. Wilkinson 12. Social and Vocal Complexity in Bats , 2003 .