Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals.

The processing of emotional expressions is fundamental for normal socialization and interaction. Reduced responsiveness to the expressions of sadness and fear has been implicated in the development of psychopathy (R. J. R. Blair, 1995). The current study investigates the ability of adult psychopathic individuals to process vocal affect. Psychopathic and nonpsychopathic adults, defined by the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; R. D. Hare, 1991), were presented with neutral words spoken with intonations conveying happiness, disgust, anger, sadness, and fear and were asked to identify the emotion of the speaker on the basis of prosody. The results indicated that psychopathic inmates were particularly impaired in the recognition of fearful vocal affect. These results are interpreted with reference to the low-fear and violence inhibition mechanism models of psychopathy.

[1]  P. Frick,et al.  Psychopathy and conduct problems in children. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  Robert D. Hare,et al.  The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised , 1996 .

[3]  R. Blair,et al.  A Selective Impairment in the Processing of Sad and Fearful Expressions in Children with Psychopathic Tendencies , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[4]  L Cipolotti,et al.  Impaired social response reversal. A case of 'acquired sociopathy'. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Emotion recognition from faces and prosody following temporal lobectomy. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[6]  S. Modgil,et al.  The cognitive-developmental approach to morality , 1976 .

[7]  P. Frick,et al.  Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: the moderating role of callous-unemotional traits. , 1997, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[8]  A. Young,et al.  Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Saul Shiffman,et al.  Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. , 1997, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[10]  J. Raven Advanced progressive matrices : sets I and II , 1962 .

[11]  C. Patrick,et al.  Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[12]  Sophie K. Scott,et al.  Impaired auditory recognition of fear and anger following bilateral amygdala lesions , 1997, Nature.

[13]  H. Soininen,et al.  Amygdaloid volume loss in psychopathy , 2000 .

[14]  David T. Lykken,et al.  The Antisocial Personalities , 1995 .

[15]  S. Mineka,et al.  Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  Stephen C. P. Wong,et al.  Is Hare's Psychopathy Checklist Reliable without the Interview? , 1988, Psychological reports.

[17]  D. Lykken,et al.  A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  M. Bradley,et al.  The psychopath as observer: emotion and attention in picture processing. , 2000, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  S. Scott,et al.  Saying it with feeling: neural responses to emotional vocalizations , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Society for Neuroscience Abstracts , 1997 .

[21]  R. Blair,et al.  Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[22]  P. Whalen Fear, Vigilance, and Ambiguity , 1998 .

[23]  R. Blair,et al.  A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath , 1995, Cognition.

[24]  Adrianna Mendrek,et al.  Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  A. Raine,et al.  Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.