Graphemes as Motor Units in the Acquisition of Writing Skills
暂无分享,去创建一个
S. Kandel | S. Valdois | O. Soler | C. Gros
[1] André Porquet,et al. L'orthographe française , 1966 .
[2] C. K. Yuen,et al. Theory and Application of Digital Signal Processing , 1978, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.
[3] M. Coltheart. Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .
[4] A. Wing. The height of handwriting , 1980 .
[5] A. Thomassen,et al. Preparation of partly precued handwriting movements: The size of movement units in handwriting , 1983 .
[6] G. V. Galen,et al. On the Simultaneous Processing of Words, Letters and Strokes in Handwriting: Evidence For a Mixed Linear and Parallel Model , 1986 .
[7] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. Movement Analysis of Repetitive Writing Behaviour of First, Second and Third Grade Primary School Children , 1986 .
[8] A. Caramazza,et al. The role of the Graphemic Buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1987, Cognition.
[9] James A. Reggia,et al. Empirically derived probabilities for grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences in english , 1987 .
[10] Monique Radeau,et al. Données statistiques sur la structure orthographique du francais , 1988 .
[11] G. V. Galen,et al. The Acquisition of Skilled Handwriting: Discontinuous Trends in Kinematic Variables , 1988 .
[12] Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al. THE PRODUCTION OF CONNECTING STROKES IN CURSIVE WRITING: DEVELOPING CO-ARTICULATION IN 8 TO 12 YEAR-OLD CHILDREN , 1989 .
[13] Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek,et al. THE ROLE OF SHORT-TERM MEMORY AND THE MOTOR BUFFER IN HANDWRITING UNDER VISUAL AND NON-VISUAL GUIDANCE , 1989 .
[14] Ching Y. Suen,et al. Computer recognition and human production of handwriting , 1989 .
[15] A. Caramazza,et al. The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.
[16] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. Perceptual-motor complexity of printed and cursive letters. , 1990 .
[17] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory ☆ , 1991 .
[18] C. Perfetti. The Representation Problem in Reading Acquisition , 1992 .
[19] Louis-Jean Boë,et al. The role of linguistics for the speed of handwriting movements : Effects of spelling uncertainty , 1993 .
[20] P. Mounoud,et al. Effects of lexicality and trigram frequency on handwriting production in children and adults. , 1993, Acta psychologica.
[21] William Badecker,et al. The structure of graphemic representations in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1994 .
[22] R. Berndt,et al. Functional pronunciation units in English words. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[23] Pierre Mounoud,et al. The influence of syllabic structure in handwriting and typing production , 1995 .
[24] D. Share. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition , 1995, Cognition.
[25] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French , 1996 .
[26] Ronald Peereman,et al. Orthographic and Phonological Neighborhoods in Naming: Not All Neighbors Are Equally Influential in Orthographic Space , 1997 .
[27] M. Coltheart,et al. Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on nonword reading , 1998 .
[28] Format distortion and word reading: The role of multiletter units , 1999 .
[29] D. Share. Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[30] R. Peereman,et al. LEXOP: A lexical database providing orthography-phonology statistics for French monosyllabic words , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[31] Arnaud Rey,et al. Graphemes are perceptual reading units , 2000, Cognition.
[32] A. Lecours,et al. The Role of Sublexical Graphemic Processing in Reading , 2000, Brain and Language.
[33] Boris New,et al. Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE , 2001 .
[34] L. Siegel,et al. Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: a four-year longitudinal study. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[35] P. H. Seymour,et al. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. , 2003, British journal of psychology.
[36] Ton Dijkstra,et al. Phonological ambiguity and context sensitivity: On sublexical clustering in visual word recognition , 2003 .
[37] Marco Zorzi,et al. NORMAL AND IMPAIRED SPELLING IN A CONNECTIONIST DUAL-ROUTE ARCHITECTURE , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[38] M. J. Tainturier,et al. Complex Graphemes as Functional Spelling Units: Evidence from Acquired Dysgraphia , 2004 .
[39] B. Rapp,et al. Complex graphemes as functional spelling units: evidence from acquired dysgraphia. , 2004, Neurocase.
[40] Richard L. Venezky,et al. In search of the perfect orthography , 2004 .
[41] Sylviane Valdois,et al. The effect of orthographic regularity on children’s handwriting production , 2005 .
[42] Arnaud Rey,et al. Graphemic complexity and multiple print-to-sound associations in visual word recognition , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[43] C. J. Álvarez,et al. Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] S. Kandel,et al. French and Spanish-speaking children use different visual and motor units during spelling acquisition , 2006 .