Hand and gender differences in the organization of aiming in 5-year-old children

The aim of the present study was to analyze the organization of aiming movements in right-handed children aged 5 years, depending on gender and hand used to perform the task. We first tested the hypothesis that aiming movements are predominantly organized in a feed-forward manner before the age of six. Using a direction pre-cueing protocol, we analyzed the effect of gender and hand used to perform the task on reaction time (RT), movement time (MT), spatial accuracy (SA) and acceleration profiles (APs) in children aged 5 years. Differences in RT between the uncued and pre-cued conditions suggest that the direction is actually specified prior to the execution of the movement at the age of five. However, the results also show significant hand and gender effects on MTs, spatial error and APs. Specifically, in girls, MT and kinematics profiles vary as a function of hand and target localization, whereas this is not the case in boys. In addition, SA is lower when aiming with the non-dominant hand in boys, but not in girls. These results suggest that multiple movement strategies are already available to the child at the age of five. Girls appear to be able to change movement strategy as a function of the constraints of the task, resulting in a stability of spatial accuracy (SA). On the contrary, the functional advantage of the right hand on MT and SA generally reported in right-handed males is not present in the boys at the age of five.

[1]  J. Mcglone,et al.  Sex differences in human brain asymmetry: a critical survey , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  S. F. Witelson The brain connection: the corpus callosum is larger in left-handers. , 1985, Science.

[3]  V B Brooks,et al.  Some examples of programmed limb movements. , 1974, Brain research.

[4]  L. Hay,et al.  Discontinuity in the Development of Motor Control in Children , 1984 .

[5]  Chantal Bard,et al.  Etude ontogénétique de la coordination visuo-manuelle , 1983 .

[6]  E. A. Roy,et al.  Kinematic Analyses of Manual Asymmetries in Visual Aiming Movements , 1994, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  T. Hatta,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in a tactile bisection task: Effects of hemispace of presentation , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  D. Glencross,et al.  Some Experimental Evidence For and Against a Parametric Conception of Movement Programming , 1989 .

[9]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[10]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Goal-Directed Movements , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  C. Redon,et al.  The control of goal-directed movements in children: Role of proprioceptive muscle afferents , 1997 .

[13]  Michel Bonnet,et al.  Specification of direction and extent in motor programming , 1982 .

[14]  L. Hay Spatial-temporal analysis of movements in children. , 1979, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  C Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Espace et structures d'espace , 1984 .

[18]  Hauert Ca,et al.  The relationship between motor function and cognition in the developmental perspective. , 1986, Italian journal of neurological sciences.

[19]  L. Hay,et al.  Accuracy of Children on an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[20]  J I Todor,et al.  Handedness and hemispheric asymmetry in the control of movements. , 1978, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  D. Elliott,et al.  Manual asymmetries in visually directed aiming. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.

[22]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[23]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Motor Performance , 1996 .

[24]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the regulation of visually guided aiming. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  D. Ulrich,et al.  Interpreting Statistical Significance and Meaningfulness in Adapted Physical Activity Research , 1998 .

[26]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Formation and lateralization of internal representations underlying motor commands during precision grip , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  C. Hofsten,et al.  The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  J. Cullen,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Goal-Directed Movement: Examination of the Motor Output Hypothesis , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[29]  A. Burton,et al.  Interhemisphere Transfer in Males and Females , 1991, Cortex.

[30]  W. Prinz,et al.  Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches , 1990 .

[31]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  D. Elliott,et al.  Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1993 .

[33]  F. Vlachos,et al.  Sex and Handedness in Development of Visuomotor Skills , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.

[34]  S. F. Witelson Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of the human corpus callosum. A postmortem morphological study. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  D Elliott,et al.  Optimizing the use of Vision in Manual Aiming: The Role of Practice , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  P. Mounoud,et al.  Development of Motor Control in the Child: Theoretical and Experimental Approaches , 1990 .

[37]  Vernon B. Brooks,et al.  Introductory lecture to session III some examples of programmed limb movements , 1974 .

[38]  K. Pearson,et al.  Control of Posture and Locomotion , 1973, Advances in Behavioral Biology.

[39]  C. Fayt,et al.  Children's Development of Reaching: Temporal and Spatial Aspects of Aimed Whole-Arm Movements , 1992, Perceptual and motor skills.

[40]  G. Pellizzer,et al.  Visuo-manual Aiming Movements in 6- to 10-Year-Old Children: Evidence for an Asymmetric and Asynchronous Development of Information Processes , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  M. Hiscock,et al.  Is there a sex difference in human laterality? III. An exhaustive survey of tactile laterality studies from six neuropsychology journals. , 1999, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[42]  C. Bard,et al.  Timing and accuracy of visually directed movements in children: control of direction and amplitude components. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Influence of the Movement Parameter to Be Controlled on Manual RT Asymmetries in Right-Handers , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[44]  K. Amunts,et al.  Interhemispheric asymmetry of the human motor cortex related to handedness and gender , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  J. Velay,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetry and Interhemispheric Transfer in Pointing Depend on the Spatial Components of the Movement , 2001, Cortex.

[46]  N Teasdale,et al.  Kinematics of aiming in direction and amplitude: a developmental study. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[47]  C. Hauert,et al.  The relationship between motor function and cognition in the developmental perspective. , 1986, Italian journal of neurological sciences.

[48]  G. Ettlinger,et al.  The accuracy of aimed movements to visual targets during development: the role of visual information. , 1986, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[49]  A. Georgopoulos Higher order motor control. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  J. Annett,et al.  Individual differences in right and left reaction time. , 1979, British journal of psychology.

[51]  R. B. Freeman,et al.  Cognition and Motor Processes , 1983 .

[52]  R. Graves,et al.  Is interhemispheric transfer related to handedness and gender? , 1988, Neuropsychologia.