On the Boundaries between Decision and Action: Effector-selective Lateralization of Beta-frequency Power Is Modulated by the Lexical Frequency of Printed Words

Current computational and neuroscientific models of decision-making posit a discrete, serial processing distinction between upstream decisional stages and downstream processes of motor-response implementation. We investigated this framework in the context of two-alternative forced-choice tasks on linguistic stimuli, words and pseudowords. In two experiments, we assessed the impact of lexical frequency and action semantics on two effector-selective EEG indexes of motor-response activation: the lateralized readiness potential and the lateralization of beta-frequency power. This allowed us to track potentially continuous streams of processing progressively mapping the evaluation of linguistic stimuli onto corresponding response channels. Whereas action semantics showed no influence on EEG indexes of motor-response activation, lexical frequency affected the lateralization of response-locked beta-frequency power. We argue that these observations point toward a continuity between linguistic processing of word input stimuli and implementation of corresponding choice in terms of motor behavior. This interpretation challenges the commonly held assumption of a discrete processing distinction between decisional and motor-response processes in the context of decisions based on symbolic stimuli.

[1]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Movement-related EEG indices of preparation in task switching and motor control , 2006, Brain Research.

[2]  Borís Burle,et al.  Linking Theoretical Decision-making Mechanisms in the Simon Task with Electrophysiological Data: A Model-based Neuroscience Study in Humans , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  S. Kelly,et al.  Internal and External Influences on the Rate of Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  M. Hallett,et al.  Asymmetric spatiotemporal patterns of event-related desynchronization preceding voluntary sequential finger movements: a high-resolution EEG study , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  T Hasbroucq,et al.  The influence of time preparation on motor processes assessed by surface Laplacian estimation , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  A low-frequency oscillatory neural signal in humans encodes a developing decision variable , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Non-Decision Time Effects in the Lexical Decision Task , 2009 .

[11]  Simon P. Kelly,et al.  Decisions are expedited through multiple neural adjustments spanning the sensorimotor hierarchy , 2017, Nature Communications.

[12]  E. Maris,et al.  Oscillatory brain responses in spoken word production reflect lexical frequency and sentential constraint , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[14]  David M. Groppe,et al.  Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[15]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[16]  Michael T. Jurkiewicz,et al.  Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: Evidence from neuromagnetic recordings , 2006, NeuroImage.

[17]  T. Rogers,et al.  The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Selective Movement Preparation Is Subserved by Selective Increases in Corticomuscular Gamma-Band Coherence , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Siegel,et al.  Motor cortex activity predicts response alternation during sensorimotor decisions , 2016, Nature Communications.

[20]  D. Balota,et al.  Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  M. Kiefer,et al.  The Sound of Concepts: Four Markers for a Link between Auditory and Conceptual Brain Systems , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Borís Burle,et al.  Using Covert Response Activation to Test Latent Assumptions of Formal Decision-Making Models in Humans , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  K M Spencer,et al.  The lateralized readiness potential: relationship between human data and response activation in a connectionist model. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[25]  S. Kelly,et al.  The classic P300 encodes a build‐to‐threshold decision variable , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Inhibition of Return Arises from Inhibition of Response Processes: An Analysis of Oscillatory Beta Activity , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Daffertshofer,et al.  A role of beta oscillatory synchrony in biasing response competition? , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  B. Burle,et al.  The Way We Do the Things We Do: How Cognitive Contexts Shape the Neural Dynamics of Motor Areas in Humans , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[29]  Christian Gerloff,et al.  Event-related desynchronization and excitability of the ipsilateral motor cortex during simple self-paced finger movements , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[30]  Simon P. Kelly,et al.  Abstract and Effector-Selective Decision Signals Exhibit Qualitatively Distinct Dynamics before Delayed Perceptual Reports , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  D. Balota,et al.  Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words , 2009 .

[32]  Tom Verguts,et al.  The Unfolding Action Model of Initiation Times, Movement Times, and Movement Paths , 2018, Psychological review.

[33]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Cross-talk between Language Processes and Overt Motor Behavior in the First 200 msec of Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[35]  H. Lau,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity over Motor Cortex Reflects Perceptual Expectations , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[37]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Human Perceptual Choice Under Focused and Divided Attention , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Bhaskar D. Rao,et al.  Newton method for the ICA mixture model , 2008, 2008 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing.

[39]  Mark Lerner,et al.  How to say no , 1997 .

[40]  D. Cheyne,et al.  Intended actions and unexpected outcomes: automatic and controlled processing in a rapid motor task , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  F. Perrin,et al.  Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  T. Stroganova,et al.  Effortful verb retrieval from semantic memory drives beta suppression in mesial frontal regions involved in action initiation , 2019, Human brain mapping.

[43]  Dejan Draschkow,et al.  Cluster-based permutation tests of MEG/EEG data do not establish significance of effect latency or location. , 2019, Psychophysiology.

[44]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. , 2004, Psychological review.

[45]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  How to say "no" to a nonword: a leaky competing accumulator model of lexical decision. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[47]  E. Maris,et al.  Beta oscillations reflect memory and motor aspects of spoken word production , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[48]  P. Brown,et al.  Lateralization of event-related beta desynchronization in the EEG during pre-cued reaction time tasks , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[49]  Derek Besner,et al.  Thinking outside the box when reading aloud: Between (localist) module connection strength as a source of word frequency effects. , 2016, Psychological review.

[50]  A. Leuthold,et al.  Beta-Band Activity during Motor Planning Reflects Response Uncertainty , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Physiological evidence for response inhibition in choice reaction time tasks , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[52]  T. Rogers,et al.  Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval , 2010, NeuroImage.

[54]  Nick Yeung,et al.  Dissociable Neural Correlates of Intention and Action Preparation in Voluntary Task Switching , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[57]  Borís Burle,et al.  Preventing (impulsive) errors: Electrophysiological evidence for online inhibitory control over incorrect responses , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[58]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[59]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[60]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A Diffusion Model Account of Criterion Shifts in the Lexical Decision Task. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[61]  Andreas K. Engel,et al.  Buildup of Choice-Predictive Activity in Human Motor Cortex during Perceptual Decision Making , 2009, Current Biology.

[62]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Neural synchrony within the motor system: what have we learned so far? , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[63]  Y. Luo,et al.  Electroencephalogram oscillations differentiate semantic and prosodic processes during sentence reading , 2010, Neuroscience.

[64]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[65]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[66]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Approaches to Analysis in Model-based Cognitive Neuroscience. , 2017, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[67]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[68]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Pre-stimulus EEG effects related to response speed, task switching and upcoming response hand , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[69]  Hiroshi Shibasaki,et al.  Temporal activation pattern of parietal and premotor areas related to praxis movements , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[70]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Comparison of different spatial transformations applied to EEG data: A case study of error processing. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[71]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Spatial enhancement of EEG data by surface Laplacian estimation: the use of magnetic resonance imaging-based head models , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[72]  E. Maris Statistical testing in electrophysiological studies. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[73]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[74]  E. Ambrosini,et al.  The adaptation of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) for Italian , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[75]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Linking EEG signals, brain functions and mental operations: Advantages of the Laplacian transformation. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[76]  M. Hallett,et al.  Event-related desynchronization in reaction time paradigms: a comparison with event-related potentials and corticospinal excitability , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[77]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[78]  Montserrat Comesaña,et al.  The adaptation of the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) for European Portuguese , 2012, Behavior research methods.

[79]  Alfons Schnitzler,et al.  Language–motor interference reflected in MEG beta oscillations , 2015, NeuroImage.

[80]  P. Gomez,et al.  Decomposing encoding and decisional components in visual-word recognition: A diffusion model analysis , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[81]  Borís Burle,et al.  Knowing when to respond and the efficiency of the cortical motor command: A Laplacian ERP study , 2006, Brain Research.

[82]  A. Riehle,et al.  The ups and downs of beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortex , 2013, Experimental Neurology.

[83]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  Syntactic Unification Operations Are Reflected in Oscillatory Dynamics during On-line Sentence Comprehension , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.