Acquisition of handwriting in children with and without dysgraphia: A computational approach

Handwriting is a complex skill to acquire and it requires years of training to be mastered. Children presenting dysgraphia exhibit difficulties automatizing their handwriting. This can bring anxiety and can negatively impact education. 280 children were recruited in schools and specialized clinics to perform the Concise Evaluation Scale for Children’s Handwriting (BHK) on digital tablets. Within this dataset, we identified children with dysgraphia. Twelve digital features describing handwriting through different aspects (static, kinematic, pressure and tilt) were extracted and used to create linear models to investigate handwriting acquisition throughout education. K-means clustering was performed to define a new classification of dysgraphia. Linear models show that three features only (two kinematic and one static) showed a significant association to predict change of handwriting quality in control children. Most kinematic and statics features interacted with age. Results suggest that children with dysgraphia do not simply differ from ones without dysgraphia by quantitative differences on the BHK scale but present a different development in terms of static, kinematic, pressure and tilt features. The K-means clustering yielded 3 clusters (Ci). Children in C1 presented mild dysgraphia usually not detected in schools whereas children in C2 and C3 exhibited severe dysgraphia. Notably, C2 contained individuals displaying abnormalities in term of kinematics and pressure whilst C3 regrouped children showing mainly tilt problems. The current results open new opportunities for automatic detection of children with dysgraphia in classroom. We also believe that the training of pressure and tilt may open new therapeutic opportunities through serious games.

[1]  Wafa Johal,et al.  Reply: Limitations in the creation of an automatic diagnosis tool for dysgraphia , 2019, npj Digital Medicine.

[2]  Ragnheidur Karlsdottir,et al.  Development of reading, spelling, and writing skills from third to sixth grade in normal and dysgraphic school children. , 1991 .

[3]  K. Feder,et al.  Handwriting development, competency, and intervention , 2007, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[4]  A. Vinter,et al.  Effects of different types of learning on handwriting movements in young children , 2010 .

[5]  R. Abbott,et al.  Treatment of Handwriting Problems in Beginning Writers: Transfer from Handwriting to Composition. , 1997 .

[6]  Sara Rosenblum,et al.  Using data visualization and signal processing to characterize the handwriting process , 2006, Pediatric rehabilitation.

[7]  Denis Alamargot,et al.  Handwriting on a tablet screen: Role of visual and proprioceptive feedback in the control of movement by children and adults. , 2019, Human movement science.

[8]  S. Cermak,et al.  Fine motor activities in elementary school: preliminary findings and provisional implications for children with fine motor problems. , 1992, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[9]  Jenny Ziviani,et al.  The Development of Graphomotor Skills , 2006 .

[10]  B. Smits-Engelsman,et al.  Fine motor deficiencies in children diagnosed as DCD based on poor grapho-motor ability. , 2001, Human movement science.

[11]  M. Fayol,et al.  Is Written Language Production more Difficult than Oral Language Production? A Working Memory Approach , 1994 .

[12]  A. W. Blöte,et al.  A Longitudinal Study on the Structure of Handwriting , 1991 .

[13]  Joerg Schulz,et al.  Development and standardization of a new handwriting speed test : The Detailed Assessment of Speed of Handwriting , 2009 .

[14]  Deborah McCutchen,et al.  From novice to expert: Implications of language skills and writing-relevant knowledge for memory during the development of writing skill , 2011 .

[15]  Marcos Faúndez-Zanuy,et al.  Identification and Rating of Developmental Dysgraphia by Handwriting Analysis , 2017, IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems.

[16]  Stefan Ehrlich,et al.  Influence of age and movement complexity on kinematic hand movement parameters in childhood and adolescence , 2008, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[17]  David Cohen,et al.  Serious games to teach social interactions and emotions to individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) , 2017, Comput. Educ..

[18]  S. Graham The Role of Production Factors in Learning Disabled Students' Compositions. , 1990 .

[19]  Patrick Groff,et al.  Who Are the Better Writers-The Left-Handed or the Right-Handed? , 1964, The Elementary School Journal.

[20]  Mariangela Genna,et al.  Development, maturation and learning influence on handwriting kinematics. , 2013, Human movement science.

[21]  Wafa Johal,et al.  Automated human-level diagnosis of dysgraphia using a consumer tablet , 2018, npj Digital Medicine.

[22]  Gerard P. van Galen,et al.  On-line size control in handwriting demonstrates the continuous nature of motor programs , 1998 .

[23]  G. Arbanas Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) , 2015 .

[24]  Ian Nimmo-Smith,et al.  DEVELOPING EFFICIENCY IN CURSIVE HANDWRITING: AN ANALYSIS OF ‘t’ CROSSING BEHAVIOUR IN CHILDREN , 1989 .

[25]  M. Fayol,et al.  Is graphic activity cognitively costly? A developmental approach , 2000 .

[26]  T. van Amelsvoort,et al.  Behavioral Outcome Effects of Serious Gaming as an Adjunct to Treatment for Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial , 2016, Journal of medical Internet research.

[27]  Annie Vinter,et al.  The impact of spatio-temporal constraints on cursive letter handwriting in children , 2008 .

[28]  R. Deuel,et al.  Developmental Dysgraphia and Motor Skills Disorders , 1995, Journal of child neurology.