Inference Generation and Text Comprehension in Bilingual Children: A Case study/Die Maak Van Afleidings En Teksbegrip by Tweetalige Kinders: 'N Gevallestudie

Introduction In order to understand text, it is essential that comprehenders draw inferences, because generally a text can only provide a certain amount of essential information, which the recipients are required to expand with inferences. Inferences that serve to fill gaps identified in a text are generated through the use of different types of knowledge bases (Keenan, Baillet & Brown 1984; Kintsch & Van Dijk 1978; Van Dijk & Kintsch 1983). Elaborative inferences embellish and amplify written or aural text as well as visual stimuli. These inferences do not help a reader and/or listener achieve comprehension but rather lead to the establishment of a rich situation model. There is an enormous number of inferences that comprehenders make; below we provide an overview of different types of inferences with the aim of positioning the subject of our current investigation within the research field. Different types of inferences There is hardly any consensus among researchers in psycholinguistics and discourse processing on the different types of inferences that readers, listeners or viewers make (Graesser, Singer & Trabasso 1994:374). Table 1 lists the most frequently cited distinctions between different types of inferences. Graesser et al. (1994:371-372) as well as Pressley and Afflerbach (1995:46-48) compile a comprehensive, rather than exhaustive, account of different types of inferences. The categories that Graesser et al. (1994:371-372) recognise include text-connecting versus knowledge-based inferences on the one hand as well as local versus global inferences on the other hand. The general distinction between text-connecting and knowledge-based inferences is commonly accepted (e.g. Cain & Oakhill 1999). According to Cain and Oakhill (1999:490), text-connecting inferences involve the integration of textual information and are hence necessary to establish cohesion between sentences. Authors such as Barnes, Dennis and Haefele-Kalvaitis (1996:217-219), Calvo (2004:54) and Bowyer-Crane and Snowling (2005:19-21) adopt the more current terms of coherence versus elaborative inferences to differentiate between text-connecting and knowledge-based inferences, respectively, while Cromley and Azevedo (2007:312) develop yet an alternative set of terminology. They employ the terms text-to-text inferences and background-to-text inferences, which are equivalent to the coherence/text-connecting inferences and elaborative/gap-filling inferences, respectively. Text-connecting inferences are needed to integrate single sentences into one cohesive textbase. Thus, they bridge gaps between different phrases and sentences on a linguistic level. A good example for a text-connecting inference is provided by Cromley and Azevedo (2007:312), who add anaphoric inferences as a sub-category of coherence and/or text-connecting and/or text-to-text inferences. Anaphoric inferences are needed to establish co-reference links among full phrases (Noun Phrases, Verb Phrases, Adjective Phrases), pro-forms and referents. Knowledge-based inferences embellish story content and amplify a story's context. They provide a fuller representation of an event, but they are not central to textual cohesion (Barnes et al. 1996:219). Because these inferences are not necessary to achieve comprehension, they are normally influenced by the accessibility of knowledge. As a specific type of knowledge-based inferences, Cain and Oakhill recognise gap-filling inferences, which make use of information from outside the text (that is, from the reader's existing background knowledge). Knowledge-based inferences generally rely on the activation of a 'mediating idea' from the reader's world knowledge, without which a text may be experienced as disjointed. Graesser, Bertus and Magliano (1995:296) maintain that for knowledge-based inferences to be constructed an interaction between the textbase and long-term memory needs to be activated. …

[1]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Strategies of discourse comprehension , 1983 .

[2]  Cem Alptekin,et al.  Cultural familiarity in inferential and literal comprehension in L2 reading , 2006 .

[3]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness , 1985 .

[4]  Agness Hara Text comprehension in multilingual children : mental representation and narrative text structure. , 2014 .

[5]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Inference during reading. , 1992, Psychological review.

[6]  Jessica Beagles-Roos,et al.  Specific impact of radio and television on children's story comprehension. , 1983 .

[7]  E. Pen A,et al.  Narrative Discourse in Bilingual Children: Language and Task Effects. , 2004, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[8]  M. Barnes,et al.  The effects of knowledge availability and knowledge accessibility on coherence and elaborative inferencing in children from six to fifteen years of age. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition , 1998 .

[10]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Toward a model of text comprehension and production. , 1978 .

[11]  F. Bartlett,et al.  Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology , 1932 .

[12]  W. Kintsch,et al.  The role of culture‐specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories∗ , 1978 .

[13]  Laurene K. Meringoff,et al.  Influence of the medium on children's story apprehension. , 1980, Journal of educational psychology.

[14]  E. Peña,et al.  Narrative discourse in bilingual children: language and task effects. , 2004, Language, speech, and hearing services in schools.

[15]  Anne Kispal,et al.  Effective teaching of inference skills for reading : literature review , 2008 .

[16]  Patricia Johnson,et al.  Effects on Reading Comprehension of Language Complexity and Cultural Background of a Text. , 1981 .

[17]  T. Trabasso,et al.  Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. , 1994, Psychological review.

[18]  İsmail Hakkı Erten,et al.  The effects of cultural familiarity on reading comprehension , 2009 .

[19]  J. Patterson Relationships of Expressive Vocabulary to Frequency of Reading and Television Experience among Bilingual Toddlers. , 2002 .

[20]  Robert Pritchard,et al.  The Effects of Cultural Schemata on Reading Processing Strategies. , 1990 .

[21]  Yukie Horiba,et al.  Comprehension processes in L2 reading: Language competence, textual coherence, and inferences , 1996, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[22]  Peter Afflerbach,et al.  Verbal Protocols of Reading: The Nature of Constructively Responsive Reading , 1996 .

[23]  P. Carrell,et al.  Schema Theory and ESL Reading Pedagogy. , 1983 .

[24]  M. Snowling,et al.  Assessing children's inference generation: what do tests of reading comprehension measure? , 2005, The British journal of educational psychology.

[25]  W. Kintsch The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction-integration model. , 1988, Psychological review.

[26]  R. Azevedo,et al.  Testing and refining the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension , 2007 .

[27]  A. Jalilifar,et al.  The Role of Cultural Nativization in Comprehension of Short Stories in EFL Reading Contexts , 2008 .

[28]  Agness Hara,et al.  Language Specific Narrative Text Structure Elements in Multilingual Children , 2014 .

[29]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  Effects of Stress and Working Memory Capacity on Foreign Language Readers' Inferential Processing During Comprehension , 2011 .

[30]  P. Leseman,et al.  The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency , 2009, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[31]  Brent A. Olde,et al.  How does the mind construct and represent stories , 2002 .

[32]  G. Salomon Television is "easy" and print is "tough": The differential investment of mental effort in learning as a function of perceptions and attributions. , 1984 .

[33]  Stephen R. Burgess,et al.  Explaining social class differences in growth of reading skills from beginning kindergarten through fourth-grade: The role of phonological awareness, rate of access, and print knowledge , 2000 .

[34]  M. Calvo Relative contribution of vocabulary knowledge and working memory span to elaborative inferences in reading , 2005 .

[35]  Shu-Fen Lai,et al.  The Functions Of Cultural Schemata In The Chinese Reading Comprehension And Reading Time Of College Students In Taiwan , 2012 .

[36]  J. Keenan,et al.  The effects of causal cohesion on comprehension and memory , 1984 .

[37]  Jane Oakhill,et al.  Comprehension skill and inference making ability:issues of causality , 1998 .

[38]  Catherine Walter,et al.  Transfer of Reading Comprehension Skills to L2 is Linked to Mental Representations of Text and to L2 Working Memory , 2004 .

[39]  P. Carrell Content and Formal Schemata in ESL Reading , 1987 .

[40]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  1. Introduction to the psychology of science text comprehension , 2002 .

[41]  W. Kintsch The Construction-Integration Model of Text Comprehension and Its Implications for Instruction , 2004 .

[42]  M. Sasaki Effects of cultural schemata on students' test-taking processes for cloze tests: a multiple data source approach , 2000 .

[43]  Jane Oakhill,et al.  Inference making ability and its relation to comprehension failure in young children , 1999 .

[44]  Beverly J. Irby,et al.  The effect of a structured story reading intervention, story retelling and higher order thinking for English language and literacy acquisition , 2010 .

[45]  Testing of Inferencing Behaviour in a Second Language , 2001 .

[46]  Jessica Taylor Piotrowski,et al.  TV as storyteller: how exposure to television narratives impacts at-risk preschoolers' story knowledge and narrative skills. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[47]  Maike Tibus,et al.  Do Films Make You Learn? Inference Processes in Expository Film Comprehension , 2013 .