Reading words, seeing style: The neuropsychology of word, font and handwriting perception

The reading of text is predominantly a left hemisphere function. However, it is also possible to process text for attributes other than word or letter identity, such as style of font or handwriting. Anecdotal observations have suggested that processing the latter may involve the right hemisphere. We devised a test that, using the identical stimuli, required subjects first to match on the basis of word identity and second to match on the basis of script style. We presented two versions, one using various computer fonts, and the other using the handwriting of different individuals. We tested four subjects with unilateral lesions who had been well characterized by neuropsychological testing and structural and/or functional MRI. We found that two prosopagnosic subjects with right lateral fusiform damage eliminating the fusiform face area and likely the right visual word form area were impaired in completion times and/or accuracy when sorting for script style, but performed better when sorting for word identity. In contrast, one alexic subject with left fusiform damage showed normal accuracy for sorting by script style and normal or mildly elevated completion times for sorting by style, but markedly prolonged reading times for sorting by word identity. A prosopagnosic subject with right medial occipitotemporal damage sparing areas in the lateral fusiform gyrus performed well on both tasks. The contrast in the performance of patients with right versus left fusiform damage suggests an important distinction in hemispheric processing that reflects not the type of stimulus but the nature of processing required.

[1]  A. Damasio,et al.  The anatomic basis of pure alexia , 1983, Neurology.

[2]  L. Harris,et al.  Effects of Typeface Characteristics on Visual Field Asymmetries for Letter Identification in Children and Adults , 1994, Brain and Language.

[3]  J. Barton Structure and function in acquired prosopagnosia: lessons from a series of 10 patients with brain damage. , 2008, Journal of neuropsychology.

[4]  Christine Chiarello,et al.  Lateralization of Lexical Processes in the Normal Brain: A Review of Visual Half-field Research , 1988 .

[5]  Max Coltheart,et al.  The MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 1981 .

[6]  Theodor Landis,et al.  Dissociation of local and global processing in visual agnosia , 1994, Vision Research.

[7]  Genjiro Hirose,et al.  Alexia without agraphia associated with right occipital lesion. , 1977, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  Jürgen M. Kaufmann,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in font-specific and abstractive priming of written personal names: Evidence from event-related brain potentials , 2006, Brain Research.

[9]  G. Assal,et al.  [Nonrecognition of familiar animals by a farmer. Zooagnosia or prosopagnosia for animals]. , 1984, Revue neurologique.

[10]  Yannick Marchand,et al.  Impaired oral reading in two atypical dyslexics: A comparison with a computational lexical-analogy model , 2005, Brain and Language.

[11]  Harry A. Whitaker,et al.  Contemporary reviews in neuropsychology , 1988 .

[12]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[13]  A. Leff,et al.  Structural anatomy of pure and hemianopic alexia , 2006, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[14]  M. Alexander,et al.  Right thalamic injury, impaired visuospatial perception, and alexia , 1982, Neurology.

[15]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Katharina Henke,et al.  Specificity of Face Recognition: Recognition of Exemplars of Non-Face Objects In Prosopagnosia , 1998, Cortex.

[17]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Sridha Sridharan,et al.  Texture for script identification , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence.

[19]  J. Richard Eiser,et al.  Research in psychology and medicine , 1979 .

[20]  F. Allard,et al.  Visual hemifield differences depend on typeface , 1976, Brain and Language.

[21]  A. Wing Variability in Handwritten Characters. , 1979 .

[22]  A. Young,et al.  Aspects of face processing , 1986 .

[23]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  R. Campbell,et al.  FACE RECOGNITION AND LIPREADING , 1986 .

[25]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Encoding in the Visual Word Form Area: An fMRI Adaptation Study of Words versus Handwriting , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Keenan,et al.  Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial configuration in prosopagnosia , 2002, Neurology.

[27]  H. Munitz,et al.  Prosopagnosia with animal face agnosia. , 1969, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[28]  T. Landis,et al.  Hemianopsie und Agnosie , 1988 .

[29]  E. Renzi,et al.  Apperceptive and Associative Forms of Prosopagnosia , 1991, Cortex.

[30]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  Font-specific priming following global amnesia and occipital lobe damage. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[31]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Letter processing in the visual system: Different activation patterns for single letters and strings , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  M F Huque,et al.  Some comments on frequently used multiple endpoint adjustment methods in clinical trials. , 1997, Statistics in medicine.

[33]  S Erdem,et al.  Alexia without either agraphia or hemianopia in temporal lobe lesion due to herpes simplex encephalitis. , 1995, Journal of neuro-ophthalmology : the official journal of the North American Neuro-Ophthalmology Society.

[34]  George L. Malcolm,et al.  Alexia With and Without Agraphia: An Assessment of Two Classical Syndromes , 2008, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[35]  D. Le Bihan,et al.  Visualizing the Neural Bases of a Disconnection Syndrome with Diffusion Tensor Imaging , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: New diffusion imaging evidence for an old concept , 2008, Cortex.

[37]  Myra A. Fernandes,et al.  Functional specificity of the visual word form area: General activation for words and symbols but specific network activation for words , 2008, Brain and Language.

[38]  Christopher J. Fox,et al.  Defining the face processing network: Optimization of the functional localizer in fMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[39]  Joseph B. Hellige,et al.  Hemispheric differences in processing handwritten cursive , 2007, Brain and Language.

[40]  L. Cohen,et al.  The neural bases of prosopagnosia and pure alexia: recent insights from functional neuroimaging , 2006, Current opinion in neurology.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  D W Corcoran,et al.  An Aspect of Perceptual Organization Involved in Reading Typed and Handwritten Words , 1970, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  Hashim Hanif,et al.  Relating visual to verbal semantic knowledge: the evaluation of object recognition in prosopagnosia. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  A. Gomori,et al.  Visual agnosia without alexia , 1984, Neurology.

[45]  E K Warrington,et al.  Prosopagnosia: A Face-Specific Disorder , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[46]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  Letter-Case-Specific Priming in the Right Cerebral Hemisphere with a Form-Specific Perceptual Identification Task , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  Prosopagnosia: Anatomic and Physiologic Aspects , 1986 .

[50]  Mariya V. Cherkasova,et al.  Face imagery and its relation to perception and covert recognition in prosopagnosia , 2003, Neurology.

[51]  J. Riddoch,et al.  Are faces special? A case of pure prosopagnosia , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[52]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Form-specific visual priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  Christina M. Frederick,et al.  Form-Specific Visual Priming in the Left and Right Hemispheres , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[54]  D. Schacter,et al.  Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  Stefan Robert Schweinberger,et al.  Magnetic Stimulation of the Right Visual Cortex Impairs Form-specific Priming , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.