Knowledge of Results for Motor Learning: Relationship Between Error Estimation and Knowledge of Results Frequency

Abstract The authors of the present study investigated the apparent contradiction between early and more recent views of knowledge of results (KR), the idea that how one is engaged before receiving KR may not be independent of how one uses that KR. In a 2 × 2 factorial design, participants (N = 64) practiced a simple force-production task and (a) were required, or not required, to estimate error about their previous response and (b) were provided KR either after every response (100%) or after every 5th response (20%) during acquisition. A no-KR retention test revealed an interaction between acquisition error estimation and KR frequencies. The group that received 100% KR and was required to error estimate during acquisition performed the best during retention. The 2 groups that received 20% KR performed less well. Finally, the group that received 100% KR and was not required to error estimate during acquisition performed the poorest during retention. One general interpretation of that pattern of results is that motor learning is an increasing function of the degree to which participants use KR to test response hypotheses (J. A. Adams, 1971; R. A. Schmidt, 1975). Practicing simple responses coupled with error estimation may embody response hypotheses that can be tested with KR, thus benefiting motor learning most under a 100% KR condition. Practicing simple responses without error estimation is less likely to embody response hypothesis, however, which may increase the probability that participants will use KR to guide upcoming responses, thus attenuating motor learning under a 100% KR condition. The authors conclude, therefore, that how one is engaged before receiving KR may not be independent of how one uses KR.

[1]  R. Kohl,et al.  Motor response variations: peripheral and central sources of error. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  J. J. Lavery Retention of simple motor skills as a function of type of knowledge of results. , 1962 .

[3]  C H Shea,et al.  The role of reduced frequency of knowledge of results during constant practice. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[4]  C. Shea,et al.  Frequent feedback enhances complex motor skill learning. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  Gabriele Wulf,et al.  Continuous Concurrent Feedback Degrades Skill Learning: Implications for Training and Simulation , 1997, Hum. Factors.

[6]  T. D. Lee,et al.  Contextual interference in movements of the same class: differential effects on program and parameter learning. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[7]  C H Shea,et al.  Summary knowledge of results. , 1991, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[8]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Optimizing summary knowledge of results for skill learning , 1990 .

[9]  J. Adams,et al.  A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  W. Shebilske,et al.  Motor Learning and Control , 1993 .

[11]  K M Newell,et al.  Motor learning without knowledge of results through the development of a response recognition mechanism. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  Richard A. Magill,et al.  Motor Learning. Concepts and Applications , 1998 .

[14]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[15]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Scheduling Information Feedback to Enhance Training Effectiveness , 1991 .

[16]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[17]  D. C. Shapiro,et al.  Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: support for the guidance hypothesis. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  J. Adams Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills. , 1987 .

[19]  K. Newell Knowledge Of Results And Motor Learning , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[20]  J C Hogan,et al.  The role of verbal estimates of movement error in ballistic skill acquisition. , 1978, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Augmenting Motor Responses With Auditory Information: Guidance Hypothesis Implications , 1995 .

[22]  Ernest T. Goetz,et al.  Feedback and practice as variables in error detection and correction. , 1973, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Frequent Augmented Feedback Can Degrade Learning: Evidence and Interpretations , 1991 .

[24]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Interpolated Activities During the Knowledge-of-Results Delay and Post-Knowledge-of-Results Interval: Effects on Performance and Learning , 1990 .

[25]  P. Dunham,et al.  Effect of Fading Knowledge of Results on Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Simple Motor Task , 1993, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  R. Schmidt A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. , 1975 .

[27]  W A Sparrow,et al.  Performance on trials without knowledge results (KR) in reduced relative frequency presentations of KR. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  H Carnahan,et al.  On the role of knowledge of results in motor learning: exploring the guidance hypothesis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Augmented Kinematic Feedback for Motor Learning. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[30]  M. Buekers,et al.  The Role of Task Experience and Prior Knowledge for Detecting Invalid Augmented Feedback while Learning a Motor Skill , 1995 .

[31]  C. Shea,et al.  Pew (1966) Revisited: Acquisition of Hierarchical Control as a Function of Observational Practice. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  H Zelaznik,et al.  Feedback in response recognition and production. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[34]  R D Tandy,et al.  Optimal length for summary knowledge of results: the influence of task-related experience and complexity. , 1996, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[35]  M. Guadagnoli,et al.  The Scheduling of Knowledge of Results. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[36]  J. A. Adams,et al.  Response feedback and motor learning. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  K. Newell,et al.  Recall and recognition in motor learning. , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  J J LAVERY,et al.  Retention of Simple Motor Skills as a Function of the Number of Trials by Which KR is Delayed , 1962, Perceptual and motor skills.

[39]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: support for the guidance hypothesis , 1989 .

[40]  D. C. Shapiro,et al.  Information Feedback for Skill Acquisition: Instantaneous Knowledge of Results Degrades Learning , 1990 .

[41]  D E Sherwood,et al.  The benefits of random variable practice for spatial accuracy and error detection in a rapid aiming task. , 1996, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[42]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Evidence for an error detection mechanism in motor skills. , 1972, Journal of motor behavior.

[43]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Reduced-Frequency Concurrent and Terminal Feedback: A Test of the Guidance Hypothesis , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[44]  B. Sidaway,et al.  Summary and frequency of KR presentation effects on retention of a motor skill. , 1991, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[45]  M G Fischman,et al.  Motor skill acquisition and retention as a function of average feedback, summary feedback, and performance variability. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.