Expected TMS excites the motor system less effectively than unexpected stimulation

The brain's response to sensory input is modulated by prediction. For example, sounds that are produced by one's own actions, or those that are strongly predicted by environmental cues, elicit an attenuated N1 component in the auditory evoked potential. It has been suggested that this form of sensory attenuation to stimulation produced by one's own actions is the reason we are unable to tickle ourselves. Here we examined whether the neural response to direct stimulation of the brain is attenuated by prediction in a similar manner. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) applied over primary motor cortex can be used to gauge the excitability of the motor system. Motor-evoked potentials (MEPs), elicited by TMS and measured in peripheral muscles, are larger when actions are being prepared and smaller when actions are voluntarily suppressed. We tested whether the amplitude of MEPs was attenuated under circumstances where the TMS pulse can be reliably predicted, even though control of the relevant motor effector was never required. Self-initiation of the TMS pulse and reliable cuing of the TMS pulse both produced attenuated MEP amplitudes, compared to those generated programmatically in an unpredictable manner. These results suggest that predictive coding may be governed by domain-general mechanisms responsible for all forms predictive learning. The findings also have important methodological implications for designing TMS experiments that control for the predictability of TMS pulses.

[1]  J. Poulet,et al.  New insights into corollary discharges mediated by identified neural pathways , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[2]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  Motor Memory: Revealing Conditioned Action Tendencies Using Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Bradley S Peterson,et al.  Deficits in Predictive Coding Underlie Hallucinations in Schizophrenia , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Plourde Auditory evoked potentials. , 2006, Best practice & research. Clinical anaesthesiology.

[5]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Effect of Aging on Motor Inhibition during Action Preparation under Sensory Conflict , 2016, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[7]  S. Kapur Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  Lars Muckli,et al.  The brain's predictive prowess revealed in primary visual cortex , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Srikantan S. Nagarajan,et al.  Motor-induced Suppression of the Auditory Cortex , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[11]  S. Rossi,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee , 2015, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  C Tomberg,et al.  Mapping early somatosensory evoked potentials in selective attention: critical evaluation of control conditions used for titrating by difference the cognitive P30, P40, P100 and N140. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[13]  Pierre-Alexandre Klein,et al.  Generic Inhibition of the Selected Movement and Constrained Inhibition of Nonselected Movements during Response Preparation , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding under the free-energy principle , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Preliminary Observations on Tickling Oneself , 1971, Nature.

[16]  ChrisD . Frith,et al.  Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[18]  Thomas J. Whitford,et al.  Act Now, Play Later: Temporal Expectations Regarding the Onset of Self-initiated Sensations Can Be Modified with Behavioral Training , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  I. M. Harris,et al.  Motor Conflict: Revealing Involuntary Conditioned Motor Preparation Using Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. , 2019, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Canonical Microcircuits for Predictive Coding , 2012, Neuron.

[21]  Nicholas B. Allen,et al.  Chronic modulation of serotonergic neurotransmission with sertraline attenuates the loudness dependence of the auditory evoked potential in healthy participants , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[22]  Judith M Ford,et al.  Dissecting corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[23]  M. Sommer,et al.  Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Modelling Non-invasive Brain Stimulation in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2022 .

[25]  Nicolas A. McNair,et al.  MagPy: A Python toolbox for controlling Magstim transcranial magnetic stimulators , 2017, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[26]  Manuel S. Seet,et al.  Associatively-mediated suppression of corticospinal excitability: A transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) study , 2019, bioRxiv.

[27]  M. Grey,et al.  Rapid Acquisition of the Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Stimulus Response Curve , 2014, Brain Stimulation.

[28]  J. Ford,et al.  Electrophysiological and diffusion tensor imaging evidence of delayed corollary discharges in patients with schizophrenia , 2010, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[30]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  Motor-evoked potentials reveal functional differences between dominant and non-dominant motor cortices during response preparation , 2018, Cortex.

[31]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Why can't you tickle yourself? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[32]  Linking cortical and behavioural inhibition: Testing the parameter specificity of a transcranial magnetic stimulation protocol , 2020, Brain Stimulation.

[33]  Michael Andres,et al.  Unsigned value prediction-error modulates the motor system in absence of choice , 2015, NeuroImage.

[34]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Neuronal coding of prediction errors. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  Christoph Mulert,et al.  Sound level dependence of the primary auditory cortex: Simultaneous measurement with 61-channel EEG and fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  M. Sommer,et al.  Corollary discharge circuits in the primate brain , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  A predictive coding perspective on autism spectrum disorders , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[38]  J. Ford,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[39]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[40]  R. Meesen,et al.  Optimization of the Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Protocol by Defining a Reliable Estimate for Corticospinal Excitability , 2014, PloS one.

[41]  Sebastiaan Overeem,et al.  Expectancy Induces Dynamic Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  An elemental model of associative learning: I. Latent inhibition and perceptual learning , 2000 .

[43]  Manuel S. Seet,et al.  Associatively-Mediated Suppression of Corticospinal Excitability: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) Study , 2019, Neuroscience.

[44]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Influence of Uncertainty and Surprise on Human Corticospinal Excitability during Preparation for Action , 2008, Current Biology.

[46]  P. Strick,et al.  Extended Practice of a Motor Skill Is Associated with Reduced Metabolic Activity in M1 , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  J. Wagemans,et al.  Precise minds in uncertain worlds: predictive coding in autism. , 2014, Psychological review.

[48]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  Ruth B. Elijah,et al.  Modifying temporal expectations: Changing cortical responsivity to delayed self-initiated sensations with training , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[50]  E. Schröger,et al.  Suppression of the auditory N1 event-related potential component with unpredictable self-initiated tones: evidence for internal forward models with dynamic stimulation. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[51]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation , 2003, Science.

[52]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[54]  K. Chiappa,et al.  Variability of motor potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.