The diagnosticity of color for emotional objects

Object classification can be facilitated if simple diagnostic features can be used to determine class membership. Previous studies have found that simple shapes may be diagnostic for emotional content and automatically alter the allocation of visual attention. In the present study, we analyzed whether color is diagnostic of emotional content and tested whether emotionally diagnostic hues alter the allocation of visual attention. Reddish-yellow hues are more common in (i.e., diagnostic of) emotional images, particularly images with positive emotional content. An exogenous cueing paradigm was employed to test whether these diagnostic hues orient attention differently from other hues due to the emotional diagnosticity. In two experiments, we found that participants allocated attention differently to diagnostic hues than to non-diagnostic hues, in a pattern indicating a broadening of spatial attention when cued with diagnostic hues. Moreover, the attentional broadening effect was predicted by self-reported measures of affective style, linking the behavioral effect to emotional processes. These results confirm the existence and use of diagnostic features for the rapid detection of emotional content.

[1]  D. Zald The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[2]  R. Dolan,et al.  Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .

[4]  T. Robbins,et al.  Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[5]  Manfred Milinski,et al.  Female sticklebacks use male coloration in mate choice and hence avoid parasitized males , 1990, Nature.

[6]  S. Segerstrom Optimism and Attentional Bias for Negative and Positive Stimuli , 2001 .

[7]  Reinhard Pekrun,et al.  The semantic red effect: Processing the word red undermines intellectual performance , 2009 .

[8]  P. J. Reber,et al.  A review of medial temporal lobe and caudate contributions to visual category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[9]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision , 2006, Biology Letters.

[10]  Lester M. Hyman,et al.  Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. , 1992 .

[11]  Derek M. Isaacowitz,et al.  Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli , 2006, Motivation and emotion.

[12]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[14]  Bruno A Olshausen,et al.  Timecourse of neural signatures of object recognition. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[15]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  K. Scherer,et al.  Spatial frequencies or emotional effects? A systematic measure of spatial frequencies for IAPS pictures by a discrete wavelet analysis , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[18]  Derrick G. Watson,et al.  Negative triangles: simple geometric shapes convey emotional valence. , 2012, Emotion.

[19]  D. Perrett,et al.  Evidence from rhesus macaques suggests that male coloration plays a role in female primate mate choice , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  N. Hagemann,et al.  When the Referee Sees Red … , 2008, Psychological science.

[21]  R. A. Hill,et al.  Psychology: Red enhances human performance in contests , 2005, Nature.

[22]  K. Doya,et al.  A Neural Correlate of Reward-Based Behavioral Learning in Caudate Nucleus: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of a Stochastic Decision Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Daniel Osorio,et al.  Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates , 2003 .

[24]  R. Wilkinson,et al.  A second look at Carver and White's (1994) BIS/BAS scales , 1998 .

[25]  Dirk Walther,et al.  Interactions of visual attention and object recognition : computational modeling, algorithms, and psychophysics. , 2006 .

[26]  K. Mogg,et al.  Selective processing of negative information: effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[27]  Philippe G Schyns,et al.  Diagnostic recognition: task constraints, object information, and their interactions , 1998, Cognition.

[28]  G. Crombez,et al.  Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? , 2004, Emotion.

[29]  Brian N. Pasley,et al.  Subcortical Discrimination of Unperceived Objects during Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Neuron.

[30]  M. Perugini,et al.  Construct validity and generalizability of the Carver–White behavioural inhibition system/behavioural activation system scales , 2001 .

[31]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Unattended Facial Expressions Asymmetrically Bias the Concurrent Processing of Nonemotional Information , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  Markus A. Maier,et al.  Basic hue-meaning associations. , 2009, Emotion.

[34]  J. Aronoff,et al.  How We Recognize Angry and Happy Emotion in People, Places, and Things , 2006 .

[35]  Vera Ferrari,et al.  Repetition and Event-related Potentials: Distinguishing Early and Late Processes in Affective Picture Perception , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  R. A. Hill,et al.  Red shirt colour is associated with long-term team success in English football , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[37]  R. Davidson Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and methodological conundrums , 1993 .

[38]  P. Lang International Affective Picture System (IAPS) : Technical Manual and Affective Ratings , 1995 .

[39]  P. Newcomer,et al.  Basic Color Terms , 1971, International Journal of American Linguistics.

[40]  Reinhard Pekrun,et al.  The Effect of Red on Avoidance Behavior in Achievement Contexts , 2009, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[41]  M. Tarr,et al.  Gender Recognition of Human Faces Using Color , 2008, Psychological science.

[42]  C. Jackson,et al.  Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: A reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective , 2006 .

[43]  Jeffrey S. Maxwell,et al.  Human Amygdala Responsivity to Masked Fearful Eye Whites , 2004, Science.

[44]  Robert H. Knapp,et al.  Attitudes toward Time and Aesthetic Choice , 1962 .

[45]  Markus A. Maier,et al.  Color and psychological functioning: the effect of red on performance attainment. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[46]  D. Weiskopf,et al.  The role of color in high-level vision , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  E. J. Wickings,et al.  Dominance, Status Signals and Coloration in Male Mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) , 2005 .

[48]  L. Leone Testing conceptual distinctions among Carver and White's (1994) BAS scales: A comment and extension on Smillie, Jackson, and Dalgleish (2006) , 2009 .

[49]  R. A. Hill,et al.  ATTRIBUTION TO RED SUGGESTS SPECIAL ROLE IN DOMINANCE SIGNALLING , 2007 .

[50]  Charles S. Carver,et al.  BIS/BAS Scales , 2011 .

[51]  Christine L Larson,et al.  The shape of threat: simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. , 2007, Emotion.

[52]  Evian Gordon,et al.  A Direct Brainstem–amygdala–cortical Dalarmt System for Subliminal Signals of Fear , 2004 .

[53]  Markus A. Maier,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Color and Psychological Functioning , 2022 .

[54]  A. Oliva,et al.  Diagnostic colours contribute to the early stages of scene categorization: Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence , 2005 .

[55]  A. Oliva,et al.  Diagnostic Colors Mediate Scene Recognition , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[56]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Handedness and sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry , 1976, Brain and Language.

[57]  R. H. Knapp,et al.  Time judgment, aesthetic preference, and need for achievement. , 1959, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[58]  David C. Zhu,et al.  Recognizing Threat: A Simple Geometric Shape Activates Neural Circuitry for Threat Detection , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  G. Legge,et al.  Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision , 1993 .

[60]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[61]  M. Bradley,et al.  Fleeting images: a new look at early emotion discrimination. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[62]  P. Whalen Fear, Vigilance, and Ambiguity , 1998 .

[63]  M. Bar,et al.  Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  J. Aronoff,et al.  The recognition of threatening facial stimuli. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[65]  B. Fredrickson The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. , 2004, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[66]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Approach-Motivated Positive Affect Reduces Breadth of Attention , 2008, Psychological science.

[67]  S. Griffith,et al.  Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[68]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Vertical and horizontal references determined by linear perspective and optic flow information , 2010 .