Two-Point Orientation Discrimination Versus the Traditional Two-Point Test for Tactile Spatial Acuity Assessment

Two-point discrimination is widely used to measure tactile spatial acuity. The validity of the two-point threshold as a spatial acuity measure rests on the assumption that two points can be distinguished from one only when the two points are sufficiently separated to evoke spatially distinguishable foci of neural activity. However, some previous research has challenged this view, suggesting instead that two-point task performance benefits from an unintended non-spatial cue, allowing spuriously good performance at small tip separations. We compared the traditional two-point task to an equally convenient alternative task in which participants attempt to discern the orientation (vertical or horizontal) of two points of contact. We used precision digital readout calipers to administer two-interval forced-choice versions of both tasks to 24 neurologically healthy adults, on the fingertip, finger base, palm, and forearm. We used Bayesian adaptive testing to estimate the participants’ psychometric functions on the two tasks. Traditional two-point performance remained significantly above chance levels even at zero point separation. In contrast, two-point orientation discrimination approached chance as point separation approached zero, as expected for a valid measure of tactile spatial acuity. Traditional two-point performance was so inflated at small point separations that 75%-correct thresholds could be determined on all tested sites for fewer than half of participants. The 95%-correct thresholds on the two tasks were similar, and correlated with receptive field spacing. In keeping with previous critiques, we conclude that the traditional two-point task provides an unintended non-spatial cue, resulting in spuriously good performance at small spatial separations. Unlike two-point discrimination, two-point orientation discrimination rigorously measures tactile spatial acuity. We recommend the use of two-point orientation discrimination for neurological assessment.

[1]  H. Kleinert,et al.  AMERICAN Society for Surgery of the hand. , 1977, The Journal of bone and joint surgery. American volume.

[2]  George A. Gescheider,et al.  Auditory and cutaneous temporal resolution of successive brief stimuli. , 1967 .

[3]  S Weinstein,et al.  Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality , 1968 .

[4]  R. Johansson,et al.  Detection of tactile stimuli. Thresholds of afferent units related to psychophysical thresholds in the human hand. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  R. Johansson,et al.  Spatial properties of the population of mechanoreceptive units in the glabrous skin of the human hand , 1980, Brain Research.

[6]  K O Johnson,et al.  Tactile spatial resolution. I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  C. B. Wynn Parry,et al.  Evaluation of sensibility and re-education of sensation in the hand , 1982 .

[8]  F. Scott The Hand: Examination and Diagnosis , 1984 .

[9]  Assh The Hand: Examination and Diagnosis , 1990 .

[10]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[11]  W. Campbell,et al.  DeJong's The neurologic examination. , 1992 .

[12]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  A psychophysical study of the mechanisms of sensory recovery following nerve injury in humans. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  Matthew Q. Patterson,et al.  Dimensions of spatial acuity in the touch sense: changes over the life span. , 1995, Somatosensory & motor research.

[14]  Emerson Foulke,et al.  Tactile acuity, aging, and braille reading in long-term blindness , 1996 .

[15]  D. J. Murray,et al.  E.H. Weber on the Tactile Senses , 2018 .

[16]  Emerson Foulke,et al.  Tactile acuity, aging, and braille reading in long-term blindness , 1996 .

[17]  G. Gescheider Psychophysics: The Fundamentals , 1997 .

[18]  K. O. Johnson,et al.  Surround suppression in the responses of primate SA1 and RA mechanoreceptive afferents mapped with a probe array. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  C. Tyler,et al.  Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold , 1999, Vision Research.

[20]  K. Sathian,et al.  Tactile perception in developmental dyslexia: a psychophysical study using gratings , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The Two-Point Threshold , 2000 .

[22]  G Lundborg,et al.  Assessment of functional outcome after nerve repair in a longitudinal cohort. , 2000, Scandinavian journal of plastic and reconstructive surgery and hand surgery.

[23]  J. Wessberg,et al.  Tactile directional sensibility: peripheral neural mechanisms in man , 2000, Brain Research.

[24]  B. Godde,et al.  Tactile Coactivation-Induced Changes in Spatial Discrimination Performance , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  P. Haggard,et al.  Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans , 2001, Current Biology.

[26]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[27]  S. Klein,et al.  Measuring, estimating, and understanding the psychometric function: A commentary , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[29]  Gregory O. Gibson,et al.  Relative roles of spatial and intensive cues in the discrimination of spatial tactile stimuli , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  George A Gescheider,et al.  Temporal gap detection in tactile channels , 2003, Somatosensory & motor research.

[31]  Ingrid M. Kanics,et al.  Tactile Acuity is Enhanced in Blindness , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Rapid Evaluation of Perceptual Thresholds The Best-Pest Calculator : A web-based application for non-expert users , 2003 .

[33]  Christina Jerosch-Herold,et al.  A Study of the Relative Responsiveness of Five Sensibility Tests for Assessment of Recovery after Median Nerve Injury and Repair , 2003, Journal of hand surgery.

[34]  Edward A. Essock,et al.  An anisotropy of human tactile sensitivity and its relation to the visual oblique effect , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  G. Lundborg,et al.  The Two-Point Discrimination Test – Time For a Re-Appraisal? , 2004, Journal of hand surgery.

[36]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[37]  Gregory O. Gibson,et al.  The effect of force and conformance on tactile intensive and spatial sensitivity , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  C. Jerosch-Herold Assessment of Sensibility after Nerve Injury and Repair: A Systematic Review of Evidence for Validity, Reliability and Responsiveness of Tests , 2005, Journal of hand surgery.

[39]  S. Harrison "The two point discrimination test--time for a reappraisal?". , 2005, Journal of hand surgery.

[40]  Gregory O. Gibson,et al.  Tactile spatial sensitivity and anisotropy , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Patrick Ragert,et al.  Tactile coactivation resets age‐related decline of human tactile discrimination , 2006, Annals of neurology.

[42]  Martin Tegenthoff,et al.  Differential effects of synchronous and asynchronous multifinger coactivation on human tactile performance , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[43]  O. Harschnitz,et al.  Revising two-point discrimination assessment in normal aging and in patients with polyneuropathies , 2008, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[44]  D. Goldreich,et al.  Diminutive Digits Discern Delicate Details: Fingertip Size and the Sex Difference in Tactile Spatial Acuity , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Daniel Goldreich,et al.  A Tactile Automated Passive-Finger Stimulator (TAPS) , 2009, Journal of visualized experiments : JoVE.

[46]  D. Boles,et al.  Laterality and sex differences in tactile detection and two-point thresholds modified by body surface area and body fat ratio , 2011, Somatosensory & motor research.

[47]  D. Goldreich,et al.  Tactile Spatial Acuity Enhancement in Blindness: Evidence for Experience-Dependent Mechanisms , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Goldreich,et al.  Short-Term Visual Deprivation Does Not Enhance Passive Tactile Spatial Acuity , 2011, PloS one.

[49]  D. Goldreich,et al.  A Physical Constraint on Perceptual Learning: Tactile Spatial Acuity Improves with Training to a Limit Set by Finger Size , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.