Human postural responses to motion of real and virtual visual environments under different support base conditions

The role of visual orientation cues for human control of upright stance is still not well understood. We, therefore, investigated stance control during motion of a visual scene as stimulus, varying the stimulus parameters and the contribution from other senses (vestibular and leg proprioceptive cues present or absent). Eight normal subjects and three patients with chronic bilateral loss of vestibular function participated. They stood on a motion platform inside a cabin with an optokinetic pattern on its interior walls. The cabin was sinusoidally rotated in anterior–posterior (a–p) direction with the horizontal rotation axis through the ankle joints (f=0.05–0.4 Hz; Amax=0.25°–4°; vmax=0.08–10°/s). The subjects’ centre of mass (COM) angular position was calculated from optoelectronically measured body sway parameters. The platform was either kept stationary or moved by coupling its position 1:1 to a–p hip position (‘body sway referenced’, BSR, platform condition), by which proprioceptive feedback of ankle joint angle became inactivated. The visual stimulus evoked inphase COM excursions (visual responses) in all subjects. (1) In normal subjects on a stationary platform, the visual responses showed saturation with both increasing velocity and displacement of the visual stimulus. The saturation showed up abruptly when visually evoked COM velocity and displacement reached approximately 0.1°/s and 0.1°, respectively. (2) In normal subjects on a BSR platform (proprioceptive feedback disabled), the visual responses showed similar saturation characteristics, but at clearly higher COM velocity and displacement values (≈1°/s and 1°, respectively). (3) In patients on a stationary platform (no vestibular cues), the visual responses were basically similar to those of the normal subjects, apart from somewhat higher gain values and less-pronounced saturation effects. (4) In patients on a BSR platform (no vestibular and proprioceptive cues, presumably only somatosensory graviceptive and visual cues), the visual responses showed an abnormal increase in gain with increasing stimulus frequency in addition to a displacement saturation. On the normal subjects we performed additional experiments in which we varied the gain of the visual response by using a ‘virtual reality’ visual stimulus or by applying small lateral platform tilts. This did not affect the saturation characteristics of the visual response to a considerable degree. We compared the present results to previous psychophysical findings on motion perception, noting similarities of the saturation characteristics in (1) with leg proprioceptive detection thresholds of approximately 0.1°/s and 0.1° and those in (2) with vestibular detection thresholds of 1°/s and 1°, respectively. From the psychophysical data one might hypothesise that a proprioceptive postural mechanism limits the visually evoked body excursions if these excursions exceed 0.1°/s and 0.1° in condition (1) and that a vestibular mechanism is doing so at 1°/s and 1° in (2). To better understand this, we performed computer simulations using a posture control model with multiple sensory feedbacks. We had recently designed the model to describe postural responses to body pull and platform tilt stimuli. Here, we added a visual input and adjusted its gain to fit the simulated data to the experimental data. The saturation characteristics of the visual responses of the normals were well mimicked by the simulations. They were caused by central thresholds of proprioceptive, vestibular and somatosensory signals in the model, which, however, differed from the psychophysical thresholds. Yet, we demonstrate in a theoretical approach that for condition (1) the model can be made monomodal proprioceptive with the psychophysical 0.1°/s and 0.1° thresholds, and for (2) monomodal vestibular with the psychophysical 1°/s and 1° thresholds, and still shows the corresponding saturation characteristics (whereas our original model covers both conditions without adjustments). The model simulations also predicted the almost normal visual responses of patients on a stationary platform and their clearly abnormal responses on a BSR platform.

[1]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[2]  T. Mergner,et al.  Human perception of horizontal trunk and head rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Hiroshi Watanabe,et al.  Visual-vestibular conflict induced by virtual reality in humans , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[4]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[5]  R. Kenyon,et al.  The influence of an immersive virtual environment on the segmental organization of postural stabilizing responses. , 2000, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[6]  M. Sanders Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .

[7]  H. Menz,et al.  Visual contribution to postural stability in older adults , 2000 .

[8]  A. J. Benson,et al.  Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. , 1989, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[9]  S. Watanabe,et al.  Postural adjustment response to depth direction moving patterns produced by virtual reality graphics. , 1999, The Japanese journal of physiology.

[10]  M. Norré Sensory interaction testing in platform posturography , 1993, The Journal of Laryngology & Otology.

[11]  C Maurer,et al.  Vestibular, visual, and somatosensory contributions to human control of upright stance , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  T. Mergner,et al.  Eye movements during combined pursuit, optokinetic and vestibular stimulation in macaque monkey , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  W. Bles,et al.  Compensation for labyrinthine defects examined by use of a tilting room. , 1983, Acta Oto-Laryngologica.

[14]  T. Brandt,et al.  Disorders of posture and gait. , 1990, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[15]  C Maurer,et al.  A multisensory posture control model of human upright stance. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[16]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .

[17]  T Mergner,et al.  Visual contributions to human self-motion perception during horizontal body rotation. , 2000, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[18]  J. Crémieux,et al.  Lateral orientation and stabilization of human stance: static versus dynamic visual cues , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  T. Mergner,et al.  Human balance control during cutaneous stimulation of the plantar soles , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  T Mergner,et al.  Which retinal and extra-retinal information is crucial for circular vection? , 2000, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[21]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Postural adjustments induced by simulated motion of differently structured environments , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  G. Barnes,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction in the control of head and eye movement: The role of visual feedback and predictive mechanisms , 1993, Progress in neurobiology.

[23]  T. Mergner,et al.  Abnormal resonance behavior of the postural control loop in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[25]  Jeffrey C. Lagarias,et al.  Convergence Properties of the Nelder-Mead Simplex Method in Low Dimensions , 1998, SIAM J. Optim..

[26]  Robert J. Peterka,et al.  Postural control model interpretation of stabilogram diffusion analysis , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  R. Peterka Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  A Straube,et al.  Visual stabilization of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  T. Mergner,et al.  Object motion perception is shaped by the motor control mechanism of ocular pursuit , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Robert J Peterka,et al.  Dynamic regulation of sensorimotor integration in human postural control. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Lewis M. Nashner,et al.  Vestibular postural control model , 1972, Kybernetik.

[33]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Proprioceptive, visual and vestibular thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in humans. , 1994, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  R. Peterka,et al.  Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural sway , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  C Maurer,et al.  Visual object localisation in space. Interaction of retinal, eye position, vestibular and neck proprioceptive information. , 2001, Experimental brain research.

[36]  W Bles,et al.  The mechanism of physiological height vertigo. , 1978, Transactions of the American Neurological Association.

[37]  Martti Juhola,et al.  Development of virtual reality stimuli for force platform posturography , 2003, Int. J. Medical Informatics.

[38]  W Bles,et al.  The mechanism of physiological height vertigo. II. Posturography. , 1980, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[39]  T Mergner Meta level concept versus classic reflex concept for the control of posture and movement. , 2004, Archives italiennes de biologie.