Younger Is Not Always Better: Development of Locomotor Adaptation from Childhood to Adulthood

New walking patterns can be learned over short timescales (i.e., adapted in minutes) using a split-belt treadmill that controls the speed of each leg independently. This leads to storage of a modified spatial and temporal motor pattern that is expressed as an aftereffect in regular walking conditions. Because split-belt walking is a novel task for adults and children alike, we used it to investigate how motor adaptation matures during human development. We also asked whether the immature pattern resembles that of people with cerebellar dysfunction, because we know that this adaptation depends on cerebellar integrity. Healthy children (3–18 years old) and adults, and individuals with cerebellar damage were adapted while walking on split belts (1:2 speed ratio). Adaptation and de-adaptation rates were quantified separately for temporal and spatial parameters. All healthy children and adults tested could learn the new timing at the same rate and showed significant aftereffects. However, children younger than 6 years old were unable to learn the new spatial coordination. Furthermore, children as old as age 11 years old showed slower rates of adaptation and de-adaptation of spatial parameters of walking. Young children showed patterns similar to cerebellar patients, with greater deficits in spatial versus temporal adaptation. Thus, although walking is a well-practiced, refined motor skill by late childhood (i.e., 11 years of age), the processes underlying learning new spatial relationships between the legs are still developing. The maturation of locomotor adaptation follows at least two time courses, which we propose is determined by the developmental state of the cerebellum.

[1]  H. Hirsch,et al.  Deficits in binocular depth perception in cats after alternating monocular deprivation , 1975, Science.

[2]  B. Gordon,et al.  Stereopsis in normal domestic cat, Siamese cat, and cat raised with alternating monocular occlusion. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  G. Orlovsky,et al.  Messages conveyed by descending tracts during scratching in the cat. II. Activity of rubrospinal neurons , 1978, Brain Research.

[4]  Y. Arshavsky,et al.  Messages conveyed by descending tracts during scratching in the cat. I. Activity of vestibulospinal neurons , 1978, Brain Research.

[5]  I. M. Gelfand,et al.  Messages conveyed by spinocerebellar pathways during scratching in the cat. II. Activity of neurons of the ventral spinocerebellar tract , 1978, Brain Research.

[6]  I. M. Gelfand,et al.  Messages conveyed by spinocerebellar pathways during scratching in the cat. I. Activity of neurons of the lateral reticular nucleus , 1978, Brain Research.

[7]  F. Zajac,et al.  Effects of stimulalating Deiters' nucleus and memdial longitudinal fasciculus on the timing of the fictive locomotor rhythm induced in cats by DOPA , 1979, Brain Research.

[8]  V. Hömberg,et al.  Development of speed of repetitive movements in children is determined by structural changes in corticospinal efferents , 1992, Neuroscience Letters.

[9]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  F. Horak,et al.  Cerebellar control of postural scaling and central set in stance. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  M. Dąmbska,et al.  Different developmental rates of selected brain structures in humans. , 1996, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[14]  M. Hallett,et al.  International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome , 1997, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[15]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  J. Baizer,et al.  Cerebellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: in vivo study. , 1999, Science.

[18]  S. D. Prentice,et al.  What guides the selection of alternate foot placement during locomotion in humans , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  S. Miller,et al.  Functional corticospinal projections are established prenatally in the human foetus permitting involvement in the development of spinal motor centres. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  C H Lücking,et al.  Development of the corticospinal system and hand motor function: central conduction times and motor performance tests , 2000, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[21]  A. Diamond Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Child development.

[22]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Impedance control and internal model formation when reaching in a randomly varying dynamical environment. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Konczak,et al.  Force adaptation transfers to untrained workspace regions in children , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  R A Scheidt,et al.  Learning to move amid uncertainty. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Tania Lam,et al.  Infants adapt their stepping to repeated trip-inducing stimuli. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Dan Nemet,et al.  Neuromotor noise limits motor performance, but not motor adaptation, in children. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Development of Force Adaptation During Childhood , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  T. Ebner,et al.  Hereditary cerebellar ataxia progressively impairs force adaptation during goal-directed arm movements. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Lori Ann Vallis,et al.  Children use different anticipatory control strategies than adults to circumvent an obstacle in the travel path , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington's disease but not cerebellar degeneration. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  S. M. Morton,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Locomotor Adaptations during Splitbelt Treadmill Walking , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[34]  D. Reisman,et al.  Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-stroke. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[35]  Julia T. Choi,et al.  Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Alexander Leemans,et al.  Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood , 2008, NeuroImage.

[37]  A. Bastian Understanding sensorimotor adaptation and learning for rehabilitation , 2008, Current opinion in neurology.

[38]  Matthew J D Taylor,et al.  The temporal-spatial and ground reaction impulses of turning gait: is turning symmetrical? , 2009, Gait & posture.

[39]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Walking flexibility after hemispherectomy: split-belt treadmill adaptation and feedback control. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  D Timmann,et al.  Adaptation to visuomotor rotation and force field perturbation is correlated to different brain areas in patients with cerebellar degeneration. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  A. Bastian,et al.  Thinking about walking: effects of conscious correction versus distraction on locomotor adaptation. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Henning Tiemeier,et al.  Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal morphometric MRI study , 2010, NeuroImage.