Repellents: Integrating Sensory Modalities

Recommendations for managing wildlife damage situations often involve application of multiple methods or techniques. The basis for such recommendations is unclear as there is little evidence that such combinations of methods work more effectively than the individual methods alone. In order to improve beyond hit or miss repellent applications, we should adopt principles exhibited in nature and develop repellent treatments based on the design of signals used in animal communications. In particular, characteristics that increase detectability, discriminability, and memorability should be identified and incorporated into repellent design. To do so, the sensory capabilities of the target species need to be more completely understood.

[1]  W. Bellingham,et al.  A parametric study of flavoured food avoidance in chicks , 1983 .

[2]  B. T. Lett Taste potentiates color-sickness associations in pigeons and quail , 1980 .

[3]  S. Kilburn,et al.  Sensory-cue enhancement of the bird repellency of methiocarb , 1983 .

[4]  B. P. Moore,et al.  The biological roles of pyrazines: evidence for a warning odour function , 1987 .

[5]  J. Mason,et al.  GENERALIZATION OF AND EFFECTS OF PRE-EXPOSURE ON COLOR-AVOIDANCE LEARNING BY RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS (AGELAIUS PHOENICEUS) , 1983 .

[6]  C. Wiklund,et al.  SURVIVAL OF DISTASTEFUL INSECTS AFTER BEING ATTACKED BY NAIVE BIRDS: A REAPPRAISAL OF THE THEORY OF APOSEMATIC COLORATION EVOLVING THROUGH INDIVIDUAL SELECTION , 1982, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[7]  M. Avery,et al.  Assessment of field hazards to birds from methiocarb applications to fruit crops , 1994 .

[8]  R. F. Westbrook,et al.  Potentiation instead of overshadowing in the pigeon. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[9]  J. L. Gittleman,et al.  Why are distasteful prey not cryptic? , 1980, Nature.

[10]  L. Clark,et al.  Odor thresholds in passerines , 1993 .

[11]  T. J. Roper,et al.  Conspicuousness of distasteful prey affects the strength and durability of one-trial avoidance learning , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[12]  T. Guilford,et al.  Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  K. Lorenz The Companion in the Bird's World , 1937 .

[14]  D. Pearson What is the adaptive significance of multicomponent defensive repertoires , 1989 .

[15]  R. Dolbeer,et al.  Identific ation and Assessment of Wildlife Damage: an Overview , 1994 .

[16]  L. Clark,et al.  Olfactory discrimination of plant volatiles by the European starling , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  M. Avery,et al.  Food avoidance by red-winged blackbirds conditioned with a pyrazine odor , 1990 .

[18]  J. Ptacek,et al.  THE DETECTION OF NEAR-ULTRAVIOLET LIGHT BY NONMIGRATORY AND MIGRATORY BIRDS , 1984 .

[19]  M. Avery Relative importance of taste and vision in reducing bird damage to crops with methiocarb, a chemical repellent , 1984 .

[20]  J. G. Rogers Responses of Caged Red-Winged Blackbirds to Two Types of Repellents , 1974 .

[21]  P. Brakefield,et al.  The relative importance of colour, taste and smell in the protection of an aposematic insect Coccinella septempunctata , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  T. Eisner,et al.  Toxicity, odor aversion, and "olfactory aposematism". , 1981, Science.

[23]  J. Russell Mason Avoidance of Methiocarb-Poisoned Apples by Red-Winged Blackbirds , 1989 .