Perspective taking and empathic concern in adolescence: gender differences in developmental changes.

Empathy is an important social skill and is believed to play an essential role in moral development (Hoffman, 2000). In the present longitudinal study, the authors investigated adolescents' development of perspective taking and empathic concern from age 13 to 18 years (mean age at Wave 1 = 13 years, SD = 0.46) and examined its association with pubertal status. Adolescents (283 boys, 214 girls) reported for 6 consecutive years on their dispositional perspective taking and empathic concern and for 4 consecutive years on pubertal status. Latent growth curve modeling revealed gender differences in levels and developmental trends. Gender differences in perspective taking emerged during adolescence, with girls' increases being steeper than those of the boys. Girls also showed higher levels of empathic concern than did boys. Whereas girls' empathic concern remained stable across adolescence, boys showed a decrease from early to middle adolescence with a rebound to the initial level thereafter. Boys who were physically more mature also reported lower empathic concern than did their less physically developed peers. The current study supports theoretical notions that perspective taking develops during adolescence as a result of cognitive development. Moreover, the results suggest that pubertal maturation plays a role in boys' development of empathic concern.

[1]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[2]  J. Snarey,et al.  Gender differences in moral development during early adolescence: The contribution of sex-related variations in maturation , 1993 .

[3]  R. Dahl,et al.  Pubertal development: correspondence between hormonal and physical development. , 2009, Child development.

[4]  Martha Frías,et al.  Are Women More Empathetic than Men? A Longitudinal Study in Adolescence , 2009, The Spanish journal of psychology.

[5]  L. Guttman A basis for scaling qualitative data. , 1944 .

[6]  R. Lynn,et al.  Testing the developmental theory of sex differences in intelligence on 12–18 year olds , 2004 .

[7]  A. Petersen,et al.  A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms , 1988, Journal of youth and adolescence.

[8]  R. Kline Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[9]  R. Selman The growth of interpersonal understanding : developmental and clinical analyses , 1980 .

[10]  M. Hoffman Empathy and moral development : implications for caring and justice , 2000 .

[11]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of children's sympathy from dispositional regulation and emotionality. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[12]  Loes Keijsers,et al.  Examining the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) Among Early and Late Adolescents and Their Mothers , 2013, Journal of personality assessment.

[13]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. , 2006 .

[14]  Rachel Karniol,et al.  Is Gender or Gender-Role Orientation a Better Predictor of Empathy in Adolescence? , 1998 .

[15]  L. Crockett,et al.  A Latent Growth Curve Analysis of Prosocial Behavior Among Rural Adolescents , 2007 .

[16]  D. Laible,et al.  Early Adolescence and Prosocial/Moral Behavior I: , 1999 .

[17]  C. Batson These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. , 2009 .

[18]  E. Crone,et al.  Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Mary Ellen Lynch,et al.  The Intensification of Gender-Related Role Expectations during Early Adolescence , 1983 .

[20]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Age Changes in Prosocial Responding and Moral Reasoning in Adolescence and Early Adulthood. , 2005, Journal of research on adolescence : the official journal of the Society for Research on Adolescence.

[21]  B. Englis,et al.  Expectations of Cooperation and Competition and Their Effects on Observers' Vicarious Emotional Responses , 1989 .

[22]  J. Eccles,et al.  Are adolescents the victims of raging hormones: evidence for activational effects of hormones on moods and behavior at adolescence. , 1992 .

[23]  B. Yildirim,et al.  A review on the relationship between testosterone and the interpersonal/affective facet of psychopathy , 2012, Psychiatry Research.

[24]  M. Hoffman,et al.  Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  D. Olweus,et al.  The Importance of Sex‐of‐Stimulus Object: Age Trends and Sex Differences in Empathic Responsiveness , 2001 .

[26]  Rex B. Kline,et al.  Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling , 1998 .

[27]  Mark H. Davis,et al.  Stability and change in adolescent self-consciousness and empathy , 1991 .

[28]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Sex Differences in Empathy and Related Capacities , 1983 .

[29]  T. Singer,et al.  The Social Neuroscience of Empathy , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  J. Piaget The Moral Judgment of the Child , 1932 .

[31]  A. Petersen,et al.  Masculinity, femininity, and sex role attitudes in early adolescence: exploring gender intensification. , 1990, Child development.

[32]  L. Pettitt Gender intensification of peer socialization during puberty. , 2004, New directions for child and adolescent development.