Hearing with an atympanic ear: good vibration and poor sound-pressure detection in the royal python, Python regius

SUMMARY Snakes lack both an outer ear and a tympanic middle ear, which in most tetrapods provide impedance matching between the air and inner ear fluids and hence improve pressure hearing in air. Snakes would therefore be expected to have very poor pressure hearing and generally be insensitive to airborne sound, whereas the connection of the middle ear bone to the jaw bones in snakes should confer acute sensitivity to substrate vibrations. Some studies have nevertheless claimed that snakes are quite sensitive to both vibration and sound pressure. Here we test the two hypotheses that: (1) snakes are sensitive to sound pressure and (2) snakes are sensitive to vibrations, but cannot hear the sound pressure per se. Vibration and sound-pressure sensitivities were quantified by measuring brainstem evoked potentials in 11 royal pythons, Python regius. Vibrograms and audiograms showed greatest sensitivity at low frequencies of 80–160 Hz, with sensitivities of –54 dB re. 1 m s–2 and 78 dB re. 20 μPa, respectively. To investigate whether pythons detect sound pressure or sound-induced head vibrations, we measured the sound-induced head vibrations in three dimensions when snakes were exposed to sound pressure at threshold levels. In general, head vibrations induced by threshold-level sound pressure were equal to or greater than those induced by threshold-level vibrations, and therefore sound-pressure sensitivity can be explained by sound-induced head vibration. From this we conclude that pythons, and possibly all snakes, lost effective pressure hearing with the complete reduction of a functional outer and middle ear, but have an acute vibration sensitivity that may be used for communication and detection of predators and prey.

[1]  O. Rieppel,et al.  A fossil snake with limbs. , 2000, Science.

[2]  B. Young,et al.  Response of western diamondback rattlesnakes Crotalus atrox to airborne sounds. , 2002, The Journal of experimental biology.

[3]  G. Békésy,et al.  Experiments in Hearing , 1963 .

[4]  B. Young Snake Bioacoustics: Toward a Richer Understanding of the Behavioral Ecology of Snakes , 2003, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[5]  H. E. Karlsen,et al.  Detection of infrasound and linear acceleration in fishes. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[6]  A. Bellairs,et al.  THE ORIGIN OF SNAKES , 1951, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[7]  G. Johnston,et al.  Vibrational Signaling in the Agonistic Interactions of Red-Eyed Treefrogs , 2010, Current Biology.

[8]  C. Carr,et al.  The auditory brainstem response in two lizard species. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  R. J. Ross,et al.  Detection of substrate vibrations by salamanders: Frequency sensitivity of the ear , 1980 .

[10]  G. Manley,et al.  Directionality of the lizard ear , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[11]  G. Manley An evolutionary perspective on middle ears , 2010, Hearing Research.

[12]  B. Ruthensteiner,et al.  Embedding 3D models of biological specimens in PDF publications , 2008, Microscopy research and technique.

[13]  Jakob Christensen-Dalsgaard,et al.  Evolution of a sensory novelty: Tympanic ears and the associated neural processing , 2008, Brain Research Bulletin.

[14]  H. Heffner,et al.  Audiograms of five species of rodents: implications for the evolution of hearing and the perception of pitch , 2001, Hearing Research.

[15]  Representation of acoustic signals in the eighth nerve of the tokay gecko II. Masking of pure tones with noise , 1996, Hearing Research.

[16]  Peggy S. M. Hill,et al.  How do animals use substrate-borne vibrations as an information source? , 2009, Naturwissenschaften.

[17]  B. Young,et al.  The use of ground-borne vibrations for prey localization in the Saharan sand vipers (Cerastes). , 2002, The Journal of experimental biology.

[18]  A. Tumarkin On the Evolution of the Auditory Conducting Apparatus–II , 1949, The Journal of Laryngology & Otology.

[19]  P. Regal,et al.  THE LOSS OF THE OPHIDIAN MIDDLE EAR , 1967, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[20]  G. Manley,et al.  Exceptional high-frequency hearing and matched vocalizations in Australian pygopod geckos , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[21]  M. Rosenzweig Auditory localization. , 1961, Scientific American.

[22]  J. Thewissen,et al.  Sensory Evolution on the Threshold: Adaptations in Secondarily Aquatic Vertebrates , 2008 .

[23]  J. Christensen-Dalsgaard,et al.  Biophysics of underwater hearing in the clawed frog, Xenopus laevis , 1995, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[24]  F. Manning Hearing in Rattlesnakes. , 1923 .

[25]  G. L. Walls Ophthalmological Implications for the Early History of the Snakes , 1940 .

[26]  O. Sand,et al.  Field studies of hearing in two species of flatfish Pleuronectes platessa (L.) and limanda limanda (L.) (family pleuronectidae). , 1974, Comparative biochemistry and physiology. A, Comparative physiology.

[27]  P. Igić,et al.  How body size affects middle-ear structure and function and auditory sensitivity in gekkonoid lizards. , 1998, The Journal of experimental biology.

[28]  S. Boissinot,et al.  Evolutionary Biology , 2000, Evolutionary Biology.

[29]  Michael S. Y. Lee,et al.  A snake with legs from the marine Cretaceous of the Middle East , 1997, Nature.

[30]  Jakob Christensen-Dalsgaard,et al.  Acoustical Coupling of Lizard Eardrums , 2008, Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology.

[31]  F. Barth Vibrationssinn und vibratorische Umwelt von Spinnen , 1986, Naturwissenschaften.

[32]  J. Christensen-Dalsgaard,et al.  Basic response characteristics of auditory nerve fibers in the grassfrog (Rana temporaria) , 1998, Hearing Research.

[33]  B. M. Johnstone,et al.  A comparative analysis of middle-ear function in non-mammalian vertebrates. , 1972, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[34]  P. Narins,et al.  Sound and vibration sensitivity of VIIIth nerve fibers in the frogs Leptodactylus albilabris and Rana pipiens pipiens , 2004, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[35]  Paul Friedel,et al.  Auditory localization of ground-borne vibrations in snakes. , 2008, Physical review letters.

[36]  P. H. Hartline,et al.  Physiological basis for detection of sound and vibration in snakes. , 1971, The Journal of experimental biology.

[37]  Jürgen Tautz,et al.  Vibrational communication in the fiddler crab, Uca pugilator , 1990, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[38]  J. Clack,et al.  The evolution of tetrapod ears and the fossil record. , 1997, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[39]  Y. Werner Temperature Effects on Inner-Ear Sensitivity in Six Species of Iguanid Lizards , 1972 .

[40]  Hearing in the African lungfish (Protopterus annectens): pre-adaptation to pressure hearing in tetrapods? , 2011, Biology Letters.

[41]  C. Berlin,et al.  Derived guinea pig compound VIIIth nerve action potentials to continuous pure tones , 1991, Hearing Research.

[42]  A TUMARKIN,et al.  The Evolution of the Auditory Conducting Apparatus , 1948, The Journal of Laryngology & Otology.

[43]  P. Coleman,et al.  Experiments in hearing , 1961 .

[44]  Richard R. Fay,et al.  The Evolutionary Biology of Hearing , 1992, Springer New York.