Unconsciously deciphering handwriting: Subliminal invariance for handwritten words in the visual word form area

Expert readers exhibit a remarkable ability to recognize handwriting, in spite of enormous variability in character shape-a competence whose cerebral underpinnings are unknown. Subliminal priming, combined with neuroimaging, can reveal which brain areas automatically compute an invariant representation of visual stimuli. Here, we used behavioral and fMRI priming to study the areas involved in invariant handwritten word recognition. Compared to printed words, easily readable handwritten words caused additional activity in ventral occipitotemporal cortex, particularly in the right hemisphere, while difficult handwriting also mobilized an attentional parietofrontal network. Remarkably, however, subliminal repetition effects were observed across printed and handwritten styles, whether easy or difficult to read, both behaviorally and in the activation of the left visual word form area (VWFA). These results indicate that the left inferotemporal VWFA possesses an unsuspected degree of fast and automatic visual invariance for handwritten words, although surprisingly this invariance can be reflected both as repetition suppression and as repetition enhancement.

[1]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  S. Sternberg Discovering mental processing stages: The method of additive factors. , 1998 .

[3]  W. Schneider,et al.  Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[4]  S. Dehaene Conscious and Nonconscious Processes:Distinct Forms of Evidence Accumulation? , 2011 .

[5]  Joseph B. Hellige,et al.  Hemispheric differences in processing handwritten cursive , 2007, Brain and Language.

[6]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  In defense of abstractionist theories of repetition priming and word identification , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[8]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[9]  Jeremy D. Schmahmann,et al.  Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[10]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Disruption to word or letter processing? The origins of case-mixing effects. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.

[12]  R. Henson Neuroimaging studies of priming , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[13]  Michal Lavidor An Examination of the Lateralized Abstractive/Form Specific Model Using MiXeD-CaSe Primes , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  Valérie Hahn-Barma,et al.  What and Where in Word Reading: Ventral Coding of Written Words Revealed by Parietal Atrophy , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Top-down modulation of unconscious 'automatic'processes: A gating framework , 2007 .

[17]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation , 1996, Science.

[18]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Dejerine,et al.  Contribution a l'etude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale , 2000 .

[20]  J B Poline,et al.  Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.

[21]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Form-specific visual priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  P. L. Tenpenny,et al.  Abstractionist versus episodic theories of repetition priming and word identification , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[24]  Marieke Longcamp,et al.  The imprint of action: Motor cortex involvement in visual perception of handwritten letters , 2006, NeuroImage.

[25]  Alex Martin,et al.  Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  B. Ans,et al.  A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. , 1998, Psychological review.

[27]  Derek Besner,et al.  On the Role of Outline Shape and Word-Specific Visual Pattern in the Identification of Function Words: None , 1989 .

[28]  Kenneth R. Paap,et al.  Word shape's in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Helen L Jamison,et al.  Morphology and the internal structure of words. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  Automatic Priming of Semantically Related Words Reduces Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways , 2008, NeuroImage.

[34]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[35]  B. Weekes Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .

[36]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral bases of subliminal and supraliminal priming during reading. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Ilhan Raman,et al.  Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency: revisited. , 2008 .

[38]  E. Darcy Burgund,et al.  Letter-Case-Specific Priming in the Right Cerebral Hemisphere with a Form-Specific Perceptual Identification Task , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  W. Singer,et al.  Better than conscious? : decision making, the human mind, and implications for institutions , 2008 .

[41]  D. Berwick,et al.  The truth about doctors' handwriting: a prospective study , 1996, BMJ.

[42]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[43]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[44]  Paul M Matthews,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  D. Schacter,et al.  Perceptual specificity in visual object priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[47]  G. M. Reicher Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.

[50]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[52]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Subliminal Convergence of Kanji and Kana Words: Further Evidence for Functional Parcellation of the Posterior Temporal Cortex in Visual Word Perception , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  C. Olson,et al.  Repetition suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex: relation to behavioral priming. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Lee Ryan,et al.  Masked word repetition results in increased fMRI signal: a framework for understanding signal changes in priming , 2002, Neuroreport.

[56]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Form-specific visual priming for new associations in the right cerebral hemisphere , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[57]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The Effects of Case Mixing on Word Recognition: Evidence from a PET Study , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Visual quality determines the direction of neural repetition effects. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.