Temporal Order Judgments Activate Temporal Parietal Junction

Perceptual temporal order judgments require an individual to determine the relative timing of two spatially separate events. Here we reveal the brain regions involved with this task. We had participants observe perceptually identical visual stimuli while conducting two different tasks: discriminating temporal order or discriminating spatial properties. By contrasting the functional magnetic resonance imaging signals during these tasks, we were able to isolate regions specifically engaged by each task. Participants observed two briefly presented rectangles. In one task, participants were instructed to report which appeared first, and, in the other, they were requested to report which rectangle was squarer. A potential confound of this study is that the temporal order judgment (TOJ) task required processing of brief events (onsets), whereas the shape task did not require temporal selectivity. To address this, we conducted a second study in which both tasks required discriminating brief events concurrent with the object onsets. The stimuli were similar to the first experiment, except a gray line was briefly superimposed on each rectangle at onset. Participants reported either which rectangle appeared first (TOJ) or which rectangle had a slightly wider gray line (shape). The first study found that the TOJ task resulted in greater bilateral activation of the temporal parietal junction (TPJ). The second revealed TOJ activation in the TPJ of the left hemisphere. This suggests that TPJ activation increases when we need to temporally sequence information. This finding supports the notion that the TPJ may be a crucial component of the “when” pathway.

[1]  C. Spence,et al.  Audiovisual temporal order judgments , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Janice J. Snyder,et al.  Spatial-temporal anisometries following right parietal damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Ki-Hyun Kim,et al.  The role of the right posterior parietal cortex in temporal order judgment , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  A. Larner False localising signs , 2003, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[5]  W. Gardner,et al.  Trigeminal neuralgia--hemifacial spasm--Paget's disease: significance of this association. , 1966, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Scott Sinnett,et al.  A dissociation between visual and auditory hemi-inattention: Evidence from temporal order judgements , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  G. Baylis,et al.  Visual extinction with double simultaneous stimulation: what is simultaneous? , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  S. Iversen,et al.  Detection of Audio-Visual Integration Sites in Humans by Application of Electrophysiological Criteria to the BOLD Effect , 2001, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[10]  R Rutschmann,et al.  Perception of Temporal Order and Relative Visual Latency , 1966, Science.

[11]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Association between auditory and visual symptoms of unilateral spatial neglect , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Quantitative analysis of attention and detection signals during visual search. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Y. Wada [Crossmodal attention between vision and touch in temporal order judgment task]. , 2003, Shinrigaku kenkyu : The Japanese journal of psychology.

[15]  Endel Põder,et al.  Illusory reversal of temporal order: the bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first , 2004, Vision Research.

[16]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Leriche,et al.  THE SURGERY OF PAIN , 1939 .

[18]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Functional MRI : an introduction to methods , 2002 .

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Incidence of Visual Extinction After Left Versus Right Hemisphere Stroke , 2007, Stroke.

[22]  Keith J. Worsley,et al.  Statistical analysis of activation images , 2001 .

[23]  R. Maurice-Williams,et al.  Tic convulsif: The association of trigeminal neuralgia and hemifacial spasm , 1973, Postgraduate medical journal.

[24]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory prior entry. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  J. Collier THE FALSE LOCALISING SIGNS OF INTRACRANIAL TUMOUR , 1904 .

[26]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[27]  Wilhelm Küker,et al.  The cortical substrate of visual extinction , 2003, Neuroreport.

[28]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  A. Paterson,et al.  Benign intracranial hypertension. I. Diagnosis and prognosis. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.5 The ‘when ’ pathway of the right parietal lobe , 2022 .

[31]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Bilateral deficits of transient visual attention in right parietal patients. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Visual extinction and prior entry: Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  D. Beaver Electron microscopy of the gasserian ganglion in trigeminal neuralgia. , 1967, Journal of neurosurgery.

[34]  R. Maurice-Williams,et al.  Mechanism of production of gait unsteadiness by tumours in the posterior fossa. , 1975, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.