Speaker perspective and reference in young children

When speakers choose a word, they choose the perspective from which they wish to present an entity or an event. In the present study we tested the hypothesis that young children accept multiple perspectives from an early age. That is, they know that two terms can refer to the same entity, as shown by their comprehension and production of multiple terms for the same referent. A total of 36 children aged 2;2 to 4;8 answered questions about pictures that required shifts in level (e.g., from cat to animal or the reverse) or domain (e.g., from dog to sailor or the reverse). Even the youngest two-year-olds managed both tasks: they understood multiple terms for the same referents and also produced second terms for referents that had already been labelled. These data support a many-perspectives view of lexical acquisition over a single-perspective view.

[1]  U. Neisser Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization , 1989 .

[2]  Michael Siegal,et al.  Misleading children: Causal attributions for inconsistency under repeated questioning ☆ , 1988 .

[3]  Words your children use : a survey of the words used by children in infants' schools with the resultant graded vocabulary , 1964 .

[4]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Conceptual and lexical hierarchies in young children , 1989 .

[5]  S. Waxman,et al.  Beyond the basics: preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[6]  J. Macnamara Names for Things: A Study in Human Learning , 1984 .

[7]  E. Clark,et al.  Learning to use compounds for contrast: data from Hebrew , 1989 .

[8]  C. Snow Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .

[9]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  Richard Scarry What Do People Do All Day , 1968 .

[11]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The Lexicon in Acquisition , 1996 .

[12]  E. Markman How children constrain the possible meanings of words. , 1987 .

[13]  R. Brown How shall a thing be called. , 1958, Psychological review.

[14]  Eve V. Clark Speaker Perspective and Lexical Acquisition , 1994 .

[15]  Pamela Blewitt,et al.  Dog versus collie: Vocabulary in speech to young children. , 1983 .

[16]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Compound Nouns and Category Structure in Young Children. , 1985 .

[17]  Marilyn Shatz,et al.  On the development of communicative understandings: An early strategy for interpreting and responding to messages , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Learning words in nonostensive contexts , 1994 .

[19]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[20]  Barbara A. Shepperson,et al.  Establishing New Subcategories: The Role of Category Labels and Existing Knowledge , 1991 .

[21]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[22]  C. Landreth The psychology of early childhood , 1958 .

[23]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Relations among Word Meanings in Early Lexical Development. , 1992 .

[24]  T. Au,et al.  The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor? , 1990, Child development.

[25]  Elizabeth F. Shipley,et al.  Mothers' use of superordinate category terms , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[26]  Maureen A. Callanan,et al.  How Parents Label Objects for Young Children: The Role of Input in the Acquisition of Category Hierarchies. , 1985 .

[27]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Two-year-olds learn words for absent objects and actions. , 1996 .

[28]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Two-Year-Olds Use Pragmatic Cues to Differentiate Reference to Objects and Actions , 1995 .

[29]  On calling things names , 1988 .

[30]  W. Merriman Some Reasons for the Occurrence and Eventual Correction of Children's Naming Errors. , 1986 .