Aging specifically impairs switching to an allocentric navigational strategy

Navigation abilities decline with age, partly due to deficits in numerous component processes. Impaired switching between these various processes (i.e., switching navigational strategies) is also likely to contribute to age-related navigational impairments. We tested young and old participants on a virtual plus maze task (VPM), expecting older participants to exhibit a specific strategy switching deficit, despite unimpaired learning of allocentric (place) and egocentric (response) strategies following reversals within each strategy. Our initial results suggested that older participants performed worse during place trial blocks but not response trial blocks, as well as in trial blocks following a strategy switch but not those following a reversal. However, we then separated trial blocks by both strategy and change type, revealing that these initial results were due to a more specific deficit in switching to the place strategy. Place reversals and switches to response, as well as response reversals, were unaffected. We argue that this specific “switch-to-place” deficit could account for apparent impairments in both navigational strategy switching and allocentric processing and contributes more generally to age-related decline in navigation.

[1]  E. Borella,et al.  The Role of Switching, Inhibition and Working Memory in Older Adults' Performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test , 2009, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[2]  T. Robbins,et al.  Lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle impair attentional set‐shifting in the rat , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Kesner,et al.  Caudate nucleus and memory for egocentric localization. , 1988, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  Walid S. Saba,et al.  ANALYSIS AND DESIGN , 2000 .

[6]  S. Resnick,et al.  Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[7]  S. Moffat,et al.  Effects of age on navigation strategy , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[8]  J. Wiener,et al.  Route repetition and route retracing: effects of cognitive aging , 2012, Front. Ag. Neurosci..

[9]  M. Ragozzino The Contribution of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Orbitofrontal Cortex, and Dorsomedial Striatum to Behavioral Flexibility , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Emery N Brown,et al.  Dynamic Analysis of Learning in Behavioral Experiments , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Erin L. Rich,et al.  Prelimbic/Infralimbic Inactivation Impairs Memory for Multiple Task Switches, But Not Flexible Selection of Familiar Tasks , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Morrison,et al.  Neuronal and morphological bases of cognitive decline in aged rhesus monkeys , 2012, AGE.

[13]  Cheryl L Grady,et al.  Age‐related differences in the functional connectivity of the hippocampus during memory encoding , 2003, Hippocampus.

[14]  Jakub Hort,et al.  Spatial navigation impairment is proportional to right hippocampal volume , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  D. Jeste,et al.  The mouse attentional-set-shifting task: A method for assaying successful cognitive aging? , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  Hans Jørgen G. Gundersen,et al.  Absolute number and size of pigmented locus coeruleus neurons in young and aged individuals , 1994, Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy.

[18]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  A computational theory of the hippocampal cognitive map. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[19]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[20]  Emery N Brown,et al.  Bayesian analysis of interleaved learning and response bias in behavioral experiments. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  D. Olton,et al.  Age-related spatial reference and working memory deficits assessed in the water maze , 1995, Neurobiology of Aging.

[22]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  Thomas Wolbers,et al.  Ageing effects on path integration and landmark navigation , 2012, Hippocampus.

[24]  D. German,et al.  Locus coeruleus cell loss in the aging human brain: A non‐random process , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[25]  Scott D. Moffat,et al.  Age differences in visual path integration , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  S. Sara,et al.  Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  R. Mccaffrey,et al.  Exploring Age-Related Decline on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test , 2008, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[28]  Matthew L Shapiro,et al.  Double dissociation and hierarchical organization of strategy switches and reversals in the rat PFC. , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  M. Geyer,et al.  Factor analysis of attentional set-shifting performance in young and aged mice , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[30]  S. Resnick,et al.  Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[31]  Emery N Brown,et al.  Analysis and design of behavioral experiments to characterize population learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Yufeng Zang,et al.  Abnormal Functional Connectivity of Hippocampus During Episodic Memory Retrieval Processing Network in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  B. L. Miller,et al.  Atrophy rates of entorhinal cortex in AD and normal aging , 2003, Neurology.

[34]  A Begega,et al.  Effects of ageing on allocentric and egocentric spatial strategies in the Wistar rat , 2001, Behavioural Processes.

[35]  N. Schuff,et al.  Age effects on atrophy rates of entorhinal cortex and hippocampus , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[36]  M. Heil,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition in younger and elderly adults: a study in a virtual environment. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[37]  Tianzi Jiang,et al.  Changes in hippocampal connectivity in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: Evidence from resting state fMRI , 2006, NeuroImage.

[38]  Douglas L Rosene,et al.  Impairment in abstraction and set shifting in aged Rhesus monkeys , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[39]  D. Compton Behavior strategy learning in rat: effects of lesions of the dorsal striatum or dorsal hippocampus , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[40]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Gray Matter Differences Correlate with Spontaneous Strategies in a Human Virtual Navigation Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Locus coeruleus neurofibrillary degeneration in aging, mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease , 2007, Neurobiology of Aging.

[42]  T. Kikusui,et al.  Age-related working memory deficits in the allocentric place discrimination task: possible involvement in cholinergic dysfunction , 1999, Neurobiology of Aging.

[43]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Spatial working memory activity of the caudate nucleus is sensitive to frame of reference , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[44]  S. Sikström,et al.  Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  G. Allen,et al.  Aging and path integration skill: Kinesthetic and vestibular contributions to wayfinding , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  A. Cuello,et al.  Changes with aging in the dopaminergic and noradrenergic innervation of rat neocortex , 2011, Neurobiology of Aging.

[47]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[48]  V. Luine,et al.  Spatial memory deficits in aged rats: contributions of monoaminergic systems , 1990, Brain Research.

[49]  J. Ackermann,et al.  Analysis and Design , 1993 .

[50]  J. Barton,et al.  Age differences in the formation and use of cognitive maps , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[51]  Andrew Thomas,et al.  WinBUGS - A Bayesian modelling framework: Concepts, structure, and extensibility , 2000, Stat. Comput..

[52]  Mark T. Waters,et al.  This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalauthorandsourcearecredited.Thislicensedoesnot permit commercial exploitation or the creation of derivative works without sp , 2009 .