Why Does Training Reduce Blind Pedestrians ’ Veering ?

Defined with respect to straight-line locomotion, to veer is to deviate from one’s intended straight-line path. In the absence of sources of guidance such as a target to walk toward or a guideline to walk along, veering appears to be nearly unavoidable. This phenomenon has long been a matter of both common and scientific curiosity. Why do sighted people tend to walk in circles when they lack visual guidance? Why do blind people tend to veer when crossing quiet roads? At a Vanderbilt University conference in 1991, I used the veering of blind pedestrians to illustrate the challenges of measuring spatially directed behavior (Guth, 1992). That conference, organized by the late Everett Hill, also linked basic research to progress in the provision of rehabilitation services to people with visual impairments, and several of the authors in this volume also participated in that conference (see Hill, 1992). In this paper, I revisit the veering problem with an emphasis on what has been learned in the intervening years about the source of the problem and about its remediation. For the former, I lean heavily on recent work by Kallie, Schrater, and Legge (in press), and for the latter, I rely on work carried out in collaboration with my colleagues, LaDuke and Gesink.

[1]  Billie Louise Bentzen,et al.  Directional guidance from audible pedestrian signals for street crossing , 2004, Ergonomics.

[2]  A Biomedical Analysis of the Walking Patterns of Blind Individuals Who Consistently Veer , 2001 .

[3]  John J. Rieser,et al.  The recalibration of rotational locomotion , 1999 .

[4]  E R Stollof,et al.  DEVELOPING CURB RAMP DESIGNS BASED ON CURB RADIUS , 2005 .

[5]  S L Chew,et al.  THE USE OF TRAFFIC SOUNDS BY BLIND PEDESTRIANS , 1987 .

[6]  D.A. Guth,et al.  Tests of Blind Pedestrians’ Use of Traffic Sounds for Street-crossing Alignment , 1989 .

[7]  A. A. Schaeffer,et al.  Spiral movement in man , 1928 .

[8]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Motor Learning and Performance , 1991 .

[9]  J. R. Glover,et al.  How Man Moves , 1973 .

[10]  D. Guth Space saving statistics: An introduction to constant error, variable error, and absolute error. , 1990 .

[11]  Hugo Bruggeman,et al.  Learning to throw on a rotating carousel: recalibration based on limb dynamics and projectile kinematics , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  David Guth,et al.  Echolocation Reconsidered: Using Spatial Variations in the Ambient Sound Field to Guide Locomotion , 1998 .

[13]  J. Barlow,et al.  Crossroads: Modern Interactive Intersections and Accessible Pedestrian Signals , 2005 .

[14]  Alan C. Scott,et al.  Accessible Pedestrian Signals , 1998 .

[15]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  D. Guth,et al.  Veering by Blind Pedestrians: Individual Differences and Their Implications for Instruction , 1995 .

[17]  A. Everett,et al.  Orientation and Mobility Techniques: A Guide for the Practitioner , 1976 .

[18]  Edward R. Stollof WAYFINDING AT INTERSECTIONS: EFFORTS TOWARD STANDARDIZATION-A JOINT WORKSHOP OF THE INSTITUTE OF TRANSPORTATION ENGINEERS AND THE U.S. ACCESS BOARD , 2005 .

[19]  Bryant J. Cratty,et al.  Movement and spatial awareness in blind children and youth , 1971 .

[20]  R. W. Levell,et al.  FOOTPRINT PATTERNS DURING WALKING , 1973 .

[21]  Janet M Barlow,et al.  Audible pedestrian signals as directional beacons , 2005 .