Importance of color in the segmentation of variegated surfaces.

We examined how variations in color and brightness are used by the visual system in distinguishing textured surfaces that differed in their first- or second-order statistics. Observers viewed a 32 x 32 array containing two types of square elements differing in chromaticity or luminance or both. The spatial distributions of the two kinds of elements were varied within the array until observers could distinguish two juxtaposed regions. At low but not at high contrast, observers are better able to distinguish regions when the elements differ only in chromaticity than when they differ only in luminance. The advantage of color at low contrasts results from the greater visibility of the arrays defined by color variation. An observer's capacity to distinguish textures defined by variations in first-order chromatic statistics is little affected by the addition of achromatic noise but is more affected by the addition of chromatic noise. The relative robustness of chromatic cues in the face of achromatic noise leaves the visual system well equipped to exploit color variations in segmenting complex scenes, even in the presence of variations in brightness. This capacity seems to depend on mechanisms that sum over large regions: When surfaces differ in their second-order statistics and cannot be distinguished by mechanisms that sum over large regions, the advantage of color is much diminished.

[1]  B Julesz,et al.  Inability of Humans to Discriminate between Visual Textures That Agree in Second-Order Statistics—Revisited , 1973, Perception.

[2]  LI ANDREA,et al.  Mechanisms Underlying Segmentation of Colored Textures , 1997, Vision Research.

[3]  V. Greenstein,et al.  Chromatic and luminance systems deficits in glaucoma , 1996, Vision Research.

[4]  B. Wandell,et al.  Matching color images: the effects of axial chromatic aberration , 1994 .

[5]  A Bradley,et al.  Contrast dependence and mechanisms of masking interactions among chromatic and luminance gratings. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[6]  Flicker sensitivity of the human red and green color mechanisms , 1975, Vision Research.

[7]  D. G. Green The contrast sensitivity of the colour mechanisms of the human eye , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  M. Gur,et al.  Isoluminant stimuli may not expose the full contribution of color to visual functioning: Spatial contrast sensitivity measurements indicate interaction between color and luminance processing , 1992, Vision Research.

[9]  P. Romano Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology. , 2000, Binocular vision & strabismus quarterly.

[10]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Color as a source of information in the stereo correspondence process , 1990, Vision Research.

[11]  Graeme R. Cole,et al.  Texture segregation with luminance and chromatic contrast , 1990, Vision Research.

[12]  O. Estévez,et al.  A spectral compensation method for determining the flicker characteristics of the human colour mechanisms. , 1974, Vision research.

[13]  D. Regan,et al.  Temporal summation and its limit for wavelength changes: an analog of Bloch's law for color vision. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  L M Hurvich,et al.  Temporal sensitivities related to color theory. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Cardinal directions of color space , 1982, Vision Research.

[16]  R Hilz,et al.  Wavelength discrimination measured with square-wave gratings. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[17]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D. Pollen,et al.  Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of neurones in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[20]  D. Brainard,et al.  Efficiency in detection of isoluminant and isochromatic interference fringes. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[21]  F. Kingdom,et al.  A comparison between colour and luminance contrast in a spatial linking task , 1992, Vision Research.

[22]  K R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contrast detection in luminance and chromatic noise. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[23]  J. Mollon "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[24]  Robert M. Boynton,et al.  Effects of field area and configuration on chromatic and border discriminations , 1987, Vision Research.

[25]  M. A. Bouman,et al.  Transfer of spatial chromaticity-contrast at threshold in the human eye. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[26]  K. Mullen The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red‐green and blue‐yellow chromatic gratings. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.