Perifoveal spatial compression.

We report a strong compression of space around a visual anchor presented in the near visual periphery (<5°). While subjects kept fixation, a salient visual stimulus (from now on referred to as "anchor") was presented, followed by a brief whole-field mask. At various times around mask onset a probe dot was flashed. Subjects estimated the position of the probe dot in relation to a subsequently presented comparison bar. The probe dot location was perceived nearly veridically when presented long before or after mask onset. However, when the probe dot was presented simultaneously with the mask it appeared shifted toward the anchor by as much as 50% of their separation. The anchor had to appear briefly before mask onset to attract the probe dot. No compression occurred when the anchor was presented long before or after the mask. When the probe dot and anchor were presented with similar brief duration, the more peripheral stimulus always shifted toward the more foveal stimulus independently of their temporal order. We hypothesize that the attraction might be explained by the summation of the neural activity distributions of probe and anchor.

[1]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability , 1984, Vision Research.

[2]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention , 1989, Vision Research.

[3]  H. Honda,et al.  Visual mislocalization produced by a rapid image displacement on the retina: Examination by means of dichoptic presentation of a target and its background scene , 1995, Vision Research.

[4]  David Burr,et al.  Suppression of the magnocellular pathway during saccades , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[6]  D. Levi,et al.  The influence of adaptation on perceived visual location , 1997, Vision Research.

[7]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Mark E. McCourt,et al.  Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulusmodulation of pseudoneglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Relative mislocalization of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  Sources of position-perception error for small isolated targets , 1999, Psychological research.

[12]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  C. Clifford,et al.  A functional angle on some after-effects in cortical vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[15]  A Straube,et al.  Mislocalization of peripheral targets during fixation , 2001, Vision Research.

[16]  Hiroyuki Sogo,et al.  Perception of relation of stimuli locations successively flashed before saccade , 2001, Vision Research.

[17]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.

[18]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[19]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  D. Kerzel Memory for the position of stationary objects: disentangling foveal bias and memory averaging , 2002, Vision Research.

[21]  Robert A Jacobs,et al.  Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[22]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.

[23]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[24]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Perisaccadic Mislocalization Orthogonal to Saccade Direction , 2004, Neuron.

[25]  F. Ostendorf,et al.  Perisaccadic mislocalization without saccadic eye movements , 2006, Neuroscience.

[26]  Regan,et al.  Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background , 2007 .

[27]  Markus Lappe,et al.  The Peri-Saccadic Perception of Objects and Space , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[28]  J. Pratt,et al.  Modulating the attentional repulsion effect. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[29]  M. Lappe,et al.  Motor signals in visual localization. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[30]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Adaptation and mislocalization fields for saccadic outward adaptation in humans , 2010 .

[31]  David Whitney,et al.  Voluntary attention modulates motion-induced mislocalization. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[32]  David Whitney,et al.  The Emergence of Perceived Position in the Visual System , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Yuki Yamada,et al.  Temporal course of position shift for a peripheral target. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[34]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Eye Position Effects in Oculomotor Plasticity and Visual Localization , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  S. Denéve,et al.  Focused visual attention distorts distance perception away from the attentional locus , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  David Whitaker,et al.  Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[37]  D. Burr,et al.  Visual perception at the time of successive saccades , 2012 .

[38]  D. Burr,et al.  Transient spatiotopic integration across saccadic eye movements mediates visual stability. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.