Principles We Talk by: Testing Dialogue Principles in Task-Oriented Dialogues

This paper takes two behavioural principles which have been suggested as explanatory models for human conversation, and tests them on a corpus of task-oriented dialogues (the HCRC Map Task Corpus). The principles chosen are Clark’s Collaborative Theory and Shadbolt’s Principle of Parsimony, which are both interested in notions of effort although they come from entirely different subfields of linguistics. The aim of the study is to compare the explanatory power of each of these principles when they are applied to real language data. Each of the principles was converted into a set of representative hypotheses about the types of behaviour which they would predict in dialogue. Then, a way of coding dialogue behaviour was developed, in order that the hypotheses could be tested on a suitably sized dataset. In particular, the coding system tried to distinguish between the levels of effort which participants used in their utterances. Finally, a series of statistical tests was undertaken to test the predictions of the hypotheses on the information generated by the coding system. The strongest support was found for the Principle of Parsimony and its associate Principle of Least Individual Effort, at the expense of the Collaborative Principle and the Principle of Least Collaborative Effort. There is certainly evidence that speakers try to minimise effort, but this seems to be occurring on an individual basis – which can be to the cost of the overall dialogue and task performance – rather than on a collaborative basis.

[1]  Amy Isard,et al.  Transaction and Action Coding in the Map Task Corpus , 1995 .

[2]  E. Schegloff Sequencing in Conversational Openings , 1968 .

[3]  Alex Lascarides,et al.  Abducing Temporal Discourse , 1992, NLG.

[4]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Grounding in communication , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[5]  Anne H. Anderson,et al.  The Hcrc Map Task Corpus , 1991 .

[6]  U. Connor,et al.  Discourse in the Professions: Perspectives from Corpus Linguistics , 2004 .

[7]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[8]  Stephen Isard,et al.  Why to speak, what to say and how to say it: Modelling language production in discourse. , 1987 .

[9]  Stephen Isard,et al.  Conversational Games within Dialogue , 1991 .

[10]  H. H. Clark Arenas of language use , 1993 .

[11]  余明忠 Speech acts across cultures , 2001 .

[12]  Martha Clark Cummings,et al.  Natural speech act data versus written questionnaire data: How data collection method affects speech act performance , 2006 .

[13]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[14]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .

[15]  Candace L. Sidner,et al.  Attention, Intentions, and the Structure of Discourse , 1986, CL.

[16]  J. Carletta,et al.  Risk-taking and recovery in task-oriented dialogue , 1996 .

[17]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding , 2004 .

[18]  Robin P. Fawcett,et al.  Monologue as a Turn in Dialogue: Towards an Integration of Exchange Structure and Rhetorical Structure Theory , 1992, NLG.

[19]  Stephanie D. Teasley,et al.  Perspectives on socially shared cognition , 1991 .

[20]  A. Clark,et al.  Introducing information in dialogues: forms of introduction chosen by young speakers and the responses elicited from young listeners , 1991, Journal of Child Language.

[21]  David R Traum,et al.  Towards a Computational Theory of Grounding in Natural Language Conversation , 1991 .

[22]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Anne H. Anderson,et al.  Forms of Introduction in Dialogues: Their Discourse Contexts and Communicative Consequences. , 1994 .

[24]  J. Sinclair,et al.  Towards an Analysis of Discourse: The English Used by Teachers and Pupils , 1975 .

[25]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Prospects for Artificial Intelligence , 1993 .

[26]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Referring as a collaborative process , 1986, Cognition.

[27]  Philip R. Cohen,et al.  Referring as a Collaborative Process , 2003 .

[28]  Michael F. Schober,et al.  Speakers, addressees, and frames of reference: Whose effort is minimized in conversations about locations? , 1995 .

[29]  F. Bargiela-Chiappini,et al.  Managing Language: The Discourse of Corporate Meetings , 1997 .

[30]  Gabriele Kasper,et al.  Communication Strategies: Psycholinguistic and Sociolinguistic Perspectives , 1997 .

[31]  P. Eckert Linguistic variation as social practice , 2000 .

[32]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Collaborating on contributions to conversations , 1987 .

[33]  R. Power The organisation of purposeful dialogues , 1979 .

[34]  Nikolas Coupland,et al.  ′Miscommunication′ and Problematic Talk , 1991 .

[35]  Etienne Wenger,et al.  Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation , 1991 .

[36]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Concealing one's meaning from overhearers , 1987 .

[37]  Jean Carletta,et al.  Assessing Agreement on Classification Tasks: The Kappa Statistic , 1996, CL.

[38]  Gwyneth Doherty-Sneddon,et al.  The Reliability of a Dialogue Structure Coding Scheme , 1997, CL.

[39]  Cognition Edited B Modelling Modelling Cognition , 1987 .

[40]  J Mullin,et al.  Interactive communication between children: learning how to make language work in dialogue , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[41]  Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs Collaborative processes of language use in conversation , 1986 .

[42]  Bethan L. Davies An empirical examination of cooperation, effort and risk in task-oriented dialogue , 1997 .

[43]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Roland Hausser,et al.  Principles of Pragmatics , 1989 .