Hand preference for pointing gestures and bimanual manipulation around the vocabulary spurt period.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Bernard Mazoyer,et al. Handedness and cerebral anatomical asymmetries in young adult males , 2006, NeuroImage.
[2] K. Nelson,et al. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .
[3] J. Vauclair,et al. Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.
[4] Ulf Liszkowski,et al. Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and helpfully provide information for a communicative partner , 2005 .
[5] E. Ringelstein,et al. Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[6] H. Benedict,et al. Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.
[7] Hélène Cochet,et al. Features of spontaneous pointing gestures in toddlers , 2010 .
[8] M. Tomasello,et al. Prelinguistic Infants, but Not Chimpanzees, Communicate About Absent Entities , 2009, Psychological science.
[9] M. Tomasello,et al. A new look at infant pointing. , 2007, Child development.
[10] G. Michel,et al. Right-handedness: a consequence of infant supine head-orientation preference? , 1981, Science.
[11] H. Neville,et al. Language Acquisition and Cerebral Specialization in 20-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] D. Le Bihan,et al. Structural Asymmetries in the Infant Language and Sensori-motor Networks , 2022 .
[13] Franco Lepore,et al. Near-infrared spectroscopy as an alternative to the Wada test for language mapping in children, adults and special populations. , 2007, Epileptic disorders : international epilepsy journal with videotape.
[14] Michael R Brent,et al. Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[15] S. Swinnen,et al. Dynamics of hemispheric specialization and integration in the context of motor control , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[16] Sophie Kern,et al. Lexicon development in French-speaking infants , 2007 .
[17] J. Fagard,et al. Unimanual and bimanual tasks and the assessment of handedness in toddlers , 2000 .
[18] D A Behrend,et al. The development of verb concepts: children's use of verbs to label familiar and novel events. , 1990, Child development.
[19] Colwyn Trevarthen,et al. Lateral Asymmetries in Infancy: Implications for the Development of the Hemispheres , 1996, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[20] J. Locke. A Theory of Neurolinguistic Development , 1997, Brain and Language.
[21] G. Butterworth,et al. Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy , 1996 .
[22] D. Rubin,et al. Comparing Correlated but Nonoverlapping Correlations , 1996 .
[23] J. Reznick,et al. Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt , 1990, Journal of Child Language.
[24] W. Hopkins,et al. Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): cross-sectional analysis. , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.
[25] Rana Esseily,et al. Handedness for grasping objects and pointing and the development of language in 14-month-old infants , 2011, Laterality.
[26] Jacqueline Nadel,et al. New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development , 1993 .
[27] I. Mcmanus,et al. The development of handedness in children , 1988 .
[28] P. Hepper,et al. Origins of fetal handedness , 1990, Nature.
[29] M. Tomasello. Origins of human communication , 2008 .
[30] K. A. Provins,et al. Early infant motor asymmetries and handedness: A critical evaluation of the evidence , 1992 .
[31] L. Lehtonen,et al. Early lexical development of Finnish children: A longitudinal study , 2008 .
[32] S. Dehaene,et al. Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.
[33] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. Gesture Paves the Way for Language Development , 2005, Psychological science.
[34] W. Hopkins. On the Other Hand: Statistical Issues in the Assessment and Interpretation of Hand Preference Data in Nonhuman Primates , 2004, International Journal of Primatology.
[35] Joanna Blake,et al. Form and function in the development of pointing and reaching gestures , 1994 .
[36] J. Dixon,et al. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 2001 .
[37] Karl Zilles,et al. Postnatal development of interhemispheric asymmetry in the cytoarchitecture of human area 4 , 1997, Anatomy and Embryology.
[38] L. Camaioni. The Emergence of Intentional Communication in Ontogeny, Phylogeny, and Pathology , 1997 .
[39] Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al. Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. , 2009, Developmental science.
[40] Sophie Kern,et al. Le compte-rendu parental au service de l'évaluation de la production lexicale des enfants français entre 16 et 30 mois , 2003 .
[41] M. Tomasello,et al. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[42] J. Fagard,et al. The effect of task constraints on infants’ (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects , 2005 .
[43] D. Ramsay. Fluctuations in Unimanual Hand Preference in Infants Following the Onset of Duplicated Syllable Babbling. , 1985 .
[44] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. , 2004, Developmental science.
[45] E Thelen,et al. Lateral biases and fluctuations in infants' spontaneous arm movements and reaching. , 1999, Developmental psychobiology.
[46] G. Michel,et al. The manifestation of infant hand-use preferences when reaching for objects during the seven--to thirteen-month age period. , 2006, Developmental psychobiology.
[47] Bob McMurray,et al. Defusing the Childhood Vocabulary Explosion , 2007, Science.
[48] J. Vauclair,et al. Relationship between manual preferences for object manipulation and pointing gestures in infants and toddlers. , 2009, Developmental science.
[49] C. Moore,et al. Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .
[50] C. Mervis,et al. Acquisition of the novel name--nameless category (N3C) principle. , 1994, Child development.
[51] Elizabeth Bates,et al. The Acquisition of Performatives Prior to Speech. , 1975 .
[52] L. Camaioni,et al. The Role of Declarative Pointing in Developing a Theory of Mind , 2004 .
[53] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Language and hand preference in early development , 1986 .
[54] Joanna Blake,et al. A cross-cultural comparison of communicative gestures in human infants during the transition to language , 2005 .
[55] Sotaro Kita,et al. On-line Integration of Semantic Information from Speech and Gesture: Insights from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[56] J. Bertoncini,et al. Before and after the vocabulary spurt: two modes of word acquisition? , 2003 .
[57] A. Young,et al. Infants' Hand Preferences for Actions and Gestures , 1985 .
[58] Maurizio Gentilucci,et al. The motor system and the relationships between speech and gesture , 2007 .
[59] J. Vauclair,et al. Hand preferences for unimanual and coordinated bimanual tasks in baboons (Papio anubis). , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[60] M. Raymond,et al. Is there geographical variation in human handedness? , 2004, Laterality.
[61] G. Butterworth,et al. Pointing and social awareness: declaring and requesting in the second year , 1996, Journal of Child Language.
[62] A. Braun,et al. Symbolic gestures and spoken language are processed by a common neural system , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[63] Michael Tomasello,et al. Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. , 2007, Developmental science.
[64] E. Pizzuto,et al. The link (and differences) between deixis and symbols in children’s early gestural-vocal system , 2005 .