How to use Psycholinguistic Methodologies for Comprehension and Production
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Heredia,et al. 1 On-line methods in bilingual spoken language research , 2002 .
[2] H. Hopp. Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing , 2006 .
[3] J Barker,et al. Semantic Factors in the Production of Number Agreement , 2001, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[4] Paola E. Dussias. SYNTACTIC AMBIGUITY RESOLUTION IN L2 LEARNERS , 2003, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.
[5] Paul C. Amrhein,et al. Phonological facilitation through translation in a bilingual picture-naming task , 2007, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[6] M. Miozzo,et al. Which words are activated during bilingual word production? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[7] Herbert Schriefers,et al. Conceptual coherence affects phonological activation of context objects during object naming. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[8] Jelmer P. Borst,et al. Stroop and picture—word interference are two sides of the same coin , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] A. Caramazza,et al. Orthographic and phonological effects in the picture–word interference paradigm: Evidence from a logographic language , 2009, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[10] Herbert Schriefers,et al. Distractor modality can turn semantic interference into semantic facilitation in the picture-word interference task: implications for theories of lexical access in speech production. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[11] John N. Williams,et al. Native and non-native processing of English wh- questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints , 2001, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[12] Kathleen M. Eberhard,et al. The Accessibility of Conceptual Number to the Processes of Subject–Verb Agreement in English☆☆☆ , 1999 .
[13] K. Bock. Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production , 1992 .
[14] David Caplan,et al. Verbal Working Memory and on-Line Syntactic Processing: Evidence from self-Paced Listening , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[15] Theodoros Marinis,et al. Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and Grammatical-SLI children , 2007 .
[16] Albert Costa,et al. Phonological activation of ignored pictures: Further evidence for a cascade model of lexical access , 2005 .
[17] Kathryn Bock,et al. Notional number agreement in English , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[18] Margarita Kaushanskaya,et al. Bilingual Language Processing and Interference in Bilinguals: Evidence From Eye Tracking and Picture Naming , 2007 .
[19] F Ferreira,et al. Exploring the use of prosody during language comprehension using the auditory moving window technique , 1996, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[20] A. Wingfield,et al. Age-Related Impairments in the Revision of Syntactic Misanalyses: Effects of Prosody , 2006, Language and speech.
[21] Qingfang Zhang,et al. Independent Effects of Orthographic and Phonological Facilitation on Spoken Word Production in Mandarin , 2009, Language and speech.
[22] B. MacWhinney,et al. Developmental differences in visual and auditory processing of complex sentences. , 2000, Child development.
[23] S. Garrod,et al. Observations on the Past and Future of Psycholinguistics , 2006 .
[24] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual's Two Lexicons Compete for Selection? , 1999 .
[25] C. Clifton,et al. The independence of syntactic processing , 1986 .
[26] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. Plausibility and grammatical agreement , 2003 .
[27] Albert Costa,et al. Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals , 1999, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[28] A. Wingfield,et al. Resource allocation during spoken discourse processing: Effects of age and passage difficulty as revealed by self-paced listening , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[29] D. O. Sears. College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology's view of human nature. , 1986 .
[30] Harald Clahsen,et al. Children's Processing of Ambiguous Sentences: A Study of Relative Clause Attachment , 2003 .
[31] G. Waters,et al. On-line syntactic processing in aphasia: Studies with auditory moving window presentation , 2003, Brain and Language.
[32] A. Rah,et al. PROCESSING OF THE REDUCED RELATIVE CLAUSE VERSUS MAIN VERB AMBIGUITY IN L2 LEARNERS AT DIFFERENT PROFICIENCY LEVELS , 2010, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.
[33] Zenzi M. Griffin,et al. Properties of Spoken Language Production , 2006 .
[34] Fernanda Ferreira,et al. Syntax and Production , 2006 .
[35] K Bock,et al. Language production: Methods and methodologies , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[36] Alan Juffs. Main Verb Versus Reduced Relative Clause Ambiguity Resolution in L2 Sentence Processing , 1998 .
[37] N. D. Jong. CAN SECOND LANGUAGE GRAMMAR BE LEARNED THROUGH LISTENING?: An Experimental Study , 2005, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.
[38] Carrie N. Jackson,et al. The processing and comprehension of wh-questions among second language speakers of German , 2009, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[39] J. Henderson,et al. Effects of lexical frequency and syntactic complexity in spoken-language comprehension: Evidence from the auditory moving-window technique. , 1996 .
[40] R. Job,et al. The picture-word interference effect is not a Stroop effect , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.