Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of Broca's Area Affects Verbal Responses to Gesture Observation

The aim of the present study was to determine whether Broca's area is involved in translating some aspects of arm gesture representations into mouth articulation gestures. In Experiment 1, we applied low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over Broca's area and over the symmetrical loci of the right hemisphere of participants responding verbally to communicative spoken words, to gestures, or to the simultaneous presentation of the two signals. We performed also sham stimulation over the left stimulation loci. In Experiment 2, we performed the same stimulations as in Experiment 1 to participants responding with words congruent and incongruent with gestures. After sham stimulation voicing parameters were enhanced when responding to communicative spoken words or to gestures as compared to a control condition of word reading. This effect increased when participants responded to the simultaneous presentation of both communicative signals. In contrast, voicing was interfered when the verbal responses were incongruent with gestures. The left stimulation neither induced enhancement on voicing parameters of words congruent with gestures nor interference on words incongruent with gestures. We interpreted the enhancement of the verbal response to gesturing in terms of intention to interact directly. Consequently, we proposed that Broca's area is involved in the process of translating into speech aspects concerning the social intention coded by the gesture. Moreover, we discussed the results in terms of evolution to support the theory [Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press] proposing spoken language as evolved from an ancient communication system using arm gestures.

[1]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review , 2007, Brain and Language.

[2]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language , 2002 .

[4]  P. T. Fox,et al.  Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing , 1988, Nature.

[5]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language [and Comments and Reply] , 1973, Current Anthropology.

[6]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[7]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Modulation of cortical activity during different imitative behaviors. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  A. Ptito,et al.  Speech-independent production of communicative gestures: Evidence from patients with complete callosal disconnection , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Gesture and the emergence and development of language , 2004 .

[10]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  V. Gallese Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  E. Bates,et al.  The cognitive hypothesis in language development. , 1987 .

[13]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Grammatical Distinctions in the Left Frontal Cortex , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  M. Hallett,et al.  Depression of motor cortex excitability by low‐frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1997, Neurology.

[16]  L. Canepari Manuale di fonetica , 2005 .

[17]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  Speech and gesture share the same communication system , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience – virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[21]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  ß Federation of European Neuroscience Societies SHORT COMMUNICATION Grasp observation influences speech production , 2022 .

[22]  H. Siebner,et al.  Effector‐independent representations of simple and complex imagined finger movements: a combined fMRI and TMS study , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Beyond nature-nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates , 2005 .

[24]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  Execution and observation of bringing a fruit to the mouth affect syllable pronunciation , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  G. Hewes Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language , 1992, Current Anthropology.

[26]  M. Diamond,et al.  Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation during Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading , 1995 .

[28]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[29]  V. Gallese Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[30]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Subthreshold low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation selectively decreases facilitation in the motor cortex , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[31]  S. Harris,et al.  Symposia of the Zoological Society of London , 1993 .

[32]  C. Frith,et al.  Dissociable neural pathways for the perception and recognition of expressive and instrumental gestures , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Induction of speech arrest and counting errors with rapid‐rate transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1991, Neurology.

[34]  M. Hallett,et al.  Effects of low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor excitability and basic motor behavior , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[35]  J. Decety,et al.  The effects of learning and intention on the neural network involved in the perception of meaningless actions. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  C M Epstein,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation: language function. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[37]  E. Bates,et al.  Chapter 4 – FIRST WORDS IN LANGUAGE AND ACTION: A QUALITATIVE LOOK , 1979 .

[38]  R T Knight,et al.  Neural representations of skilled movement. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  M. Studdert-Kennedy Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .

[40]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[41]  K. Meador,et al.  Optimum stimulus parameters for lateralized suppression of speech with magnetic brain stimulation , 1996, Neurology.

[42]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Modulation of corticospinal excitability by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. , 1992, Science.

[44]  S. Bookheimer Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[45]  N. Masataka Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: late onset of hand banging as a possible rate–limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling , 2001 .

[46]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Grasp with hand and mouth: a kinematic study on healthy subjects. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain-behaviour relationship by induction of 'virtual lesions'. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[52]  F. Dick,et al.  Language, gesture, and the developing brain. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[53]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy , 2004, NeuroImage.

[54]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[55]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[57]  Ina Č Užgiris,et al.  Infant performance and experience : new findings with the ordinal scales , 1987 .

[58]  Sherman Wilcox,et al.  Gesture and the Nature of Language , 1995 .

[59]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[60]  Alice C. Roy,et al.  Action observation and speech production: study on children and adults , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  V. Walsh,et al.  TMS Produces Two Dissociable Types of Speech Disruption , 2001, NeuroImage.

[62]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The role of gesture in communication and thinking , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[63]  V. Nikouline,et al.  The role of the coil click in TMS assessed with simultaneous EEG , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[64]  M. Hallett,et al.  Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Manual actions affect vocalizations of infants , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  Van Hooff,et al.  The Facial Displays of the Catarrhine Monkeys and Apes. , 1967 .