Vocal behaviour of Norwegian killer whales during carousel and seiner foraging on spring-spawning herring

Norwegian killer whales (Orcinus orca) use different techniques to forage on spring-spawning herring. Two of the commonly observed techniques are carousel feeding, a cooperative feeding method, and seiner feeding, a noncooperative method. During seiner foraging, large groups of whales forage on herring discards around the nets or on discarded by-catch of fishing boats. Very little is known about the acoustic behaviour during these foraging contexts. The aim of this study was to examine possible differences in killer whale acoustic behaviour during both foraging contexts using simple sound analysis techniques. Calling, echolocation, and tail-slap activities were measured and compared between foraging contexts. Of these, calling and tail-slap activities were higher during carousel feeding, whereas echolocation activity increased with the number of individuals, irrespective of foraging context. No call types were used exclusively during a particular foraging context. A difference in mean occurrence of one call type was detected; call type N21 occurred more often during seiner foraging than during carousel foraging contexts. We suggest that the sequence of call types, rather than the use of isolated call types, is of greater importance in the coordination of group movements during carousel foraging.

[1]  H. Pulliam,et al.  On the advantages of flocking. , 1973, Journal of theoretical biology.

[2]  T. Caraco,et al.  Ecological Determinants of Group Sizes of Foraging Lions , 1975, The American Naturalist.

[3]  E. Zimen,et al.  On the regulation of pack size in wolves. , 1976, Zeitschrift fur Tierphysiologie, Tierernahrung und Futtermittelkunde.

[4]  K. S. Norris,et al.  The Structure and Functions of Cetacean Schools. , 1979 .

[5]  James H. W. Hain,et al.  Vocalizations and Feeding Behavior of the Killer Whale (Orcinus orca) , 1979 .

[6]  R. Underwood Vigilance Behaviour in Grazing African Antelopes , 1982 .

[7]  T. Caraco,et al.  Vigilance behaviour in house sparrow flocks , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  W. Dittus Toque macaque food calls: Semantic communication concerning food distribution in the environment , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  M. Elgar House sparrows establish foraging flocks by giving chirrup calls if the resources are divisible , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  Kenneth S. Norris,et al.  Loud Impulse Sounds in Odontocete Predation and Social Behavior , 1988 .

[11]  J. Ford,et al.  Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia , 1989 .

[12]  M. Elgar,et al.  PREDATOR VIGILANCE AND GROUP SIZE IN MAMMALS AND BIRDS: A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE , 1989, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[13]  J. Ford,et al.  Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia , 1991 .

[14]  J. Parrish Do Predators 'Shape' Fish Schools: Interactions Between Predators and Their Schooling Prey , 1991 .

[15]  Charles R. Brown,et al.  Food-sharing signals among socially foraging cliff swallows , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  A. Hoelzel Foraging behaviour and social group dynamics in Puget Sound killer whales , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  Fernando Ugarte,et al.  Surface and underwater observations of cooperatively feeding killer whales in northern Norway , 1993 .

[18]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: the group-size effect , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  S. Creel,et al.  Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  W. Richardson Marine Mammals and Noise , 1995 .

[21]  Hanne Strager Pod-specific call repertoires and compound calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca Linnaeus, 1758, in the waters of northern Norway , 1995 .

[22]  J. Ford,et al.  The mixed blessing of echolocation: differences in sonar use by fish-eating and mammal-eating killer whales , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  I. Christensen,et al.  Occurrence and diet of killer whales in northern Norway: seasonal patterns relative to the distribution and abundance of Norwegian spring-spawning herring , 1996 .

[24]  R. W. Baird,et al.  Ecological and social determinants of group size in transient killer whales , 1996 .

[25]  G. Roberts Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  S. Leatherwood,et al.  Cetacean Interactions with Trawls: A Preliminary Review , 1997 .

[27]  J. Mann,et al.  Social evolution in toothed whales. , 1998, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[28]  R. W. Baird,et al.  Social organization of mammal-eating killer whales : group stability and dispersal patterns , 2000 .

[29]  E. Ogam,et al.  Killer whales (Orcinus orca) feeding on schooling herring (Clupea harengus) using underwater tail-slaps: kinematic analyses of field observations. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.

[30]  J. Ford,et al.  Characteristics of whistles from the acoustic repertoire of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  P. Corkeron,et al.  Trawling and bottlenose dolphins' social structure , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  J. Ford,et al.  Cultural transmission within maternal lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Frank Thomsen,et al.  On the communicative significance of whistles in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca) , 2002, Naturwissenschaften.

[34]  G. Huse,et al.  Modelling changes in migration pattern of herring: collective behaviour and numerical domination , 2002 .