Multivoicedness and univocality in classroom discourse: an example from theory of matter

The analyses of talk amongst students in three episodes, video recorded from a teaching sequence on the particle model for Brazilian students (age 14‐15) is presented. The analysis has two dimensions: the discoursive dimension is an attempt to understand how meanings are constructed in the classroom as a result of discoursive interactions; the conceptual dimension comprises the description of the patterns of evolution of ideas of matter based on the notion of a conceptual profile of matter, in which conceptual and historical features of the atom concept are profiled with pupils’ and adolescents’ ideas on atomism. From the students talk we can hear the interaction among different ‘voices’, representing different zones of a conceptual profile of matter. In this interaction, the students resort to both ‘authoritative’ and ‘internally persuasive’ discourses. The analysis emphasizes the role of these two types of discourse and of epistemological and ontological obstacles in the construction of scientific meani...

[1]  Jon Ogborn,et al.  Explaining Science in the Classroom , 1996 .

[2]  E. Mortimer,et al.  Conceptual change or Conceptual Profile change? , 1995 .

[3]  Neil Mercer,et al.  The Guided Construction of Knowledge: Talk Amongst Teachers and Learners , 1995 .

[4]  Pamela J. Garnett,et al.  Students' Alternative Conceptions in Chemistry: A Review of Research and Implications for Teaching and Learning , 1995 .

[5]  Y. Lotman The Text within the Text , 1988, PMLA/Publications of the Modern Language Association of America.

[6]  Alan K. Griffiths,et al.  Grade-12 Students' Misconceptions Relating to Fundamental Characteristics of Atoms and Molecules. , 1992 .

[7]  D. Kuhn Thinking as Argument , 1992 .

[8]  C. Sutton,et al.  Words, Science and Learning , 1992 .

[9]  J. Wertsch Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action , 1992 .

[10]  R. Stavy Children's conception of changes in the state of matter: From liquid (or solid) to gas , 1990 .

[11]  J. Lemke Talking Science: Language, Learning, and Values , 1990 .

[12]  Denis Newman,et al.  The Construction Zone: Working for Cognitive Change in School , 1989 .

[13]  Michael Halliday,et al.  Some grammatical problems in scientific English , 1989 .

[14]  Ruth Stavy Children's Conception of Gas. , 1988 .

[15]  Michael Billig,et al.  Arguing and Thinking: A Rhetorical Approach to Social Psychology , 1987 .

[16]  D. Westgate,et al.  Investigating classroom talk , 1987 .

[17]  Children's ideas about ‘solid’ and ‘liquid’ , 1985 .

[18]  F. Marton Phenomenography — Describing conceptions of the world around us , 1981 .

[19]  Joseph Nussbaum,et al.  Pupils' understanding of the particulate nature of matter: A cross‐age study , 1981 .

[20]  Joseph Nussbaum,et al.  Junior high school pupils' understanding of the particulate nature of matter: An interview study , 1978 .

[21]  L. Vygotsky Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press , 1978 .

[22]  L. S. Vygotskiĭ,et al.  Mind in society : the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[23]  V. Voloshinov Marxism and the philosophy of language , 1973 .

[24]  Misconceptions of Selected Science Concepts Held by Elementary School Students. , 1972 .

[25]  K. Popper,et al.  Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach. , 1975 .

[26]  D. Knight Classical scientific papers : chemistry , 1968 .

[27]  H. J. Koren,et al.  From Atomos to Atom : The History of the Concept Atom, Philosophical Series, I , 1954 .

[28]  Le développement des quantités chez l'enfant : conservation et atomisme , 1941 .

[29]  N. Bohr II - Can Quantum-Mechanical Description of Physical Reality be Considered Complete? , 1935 .

[30]  Albert Einstein,et al.  Can Quantum-Mechanical Description of Physical Reality Be Considered Complete? , 1935 .

[31]  J. P. Kelly A Short History of Atomism , 1934 .

[32]  G. Bachelard Les intuitions atomistiques : essai de classification , 1933 .