The objective conjugation in Hungarian: agreement without phi-features

Verbal agreement is normally in person, number and gender, but Hungarian verbs agree with their objects in definiteness instead: a Hungarian verb appears in the objective conjugation when it governs a definite object. The sensitivity of the objective conjugation suffixes to the definiteness of the object has been attributed to the supposition that they function as incorporated object pronouns (Szamosi 1974; den Dikken 2006), but we argue instead that they are agreement markers registering the object’s formal, not semantic, definiteness. Evidence comes from anaphoric binding, null anaphora (pro-drop), extraction islands, and the insensitivity of the objective conjugation to any of the factors known to condition the use of affixal and clitic pronominals. We propose that the objective conjugation is triggered by a formal definiteness feature and offer a grammar that determines, for a given complement of a verb, whether it triggers the objective conjugation on the verb. Although the objective conjugation suffixes are not pronominal, they are thought to derive historically from incorporated pronouns (Hajdú 1972), and we suggest that while referentiality and ϕ-features were largely lost, an association with topicality led to a formal condition of object definiteness. The result is an agreement marker that lacks ϕ-features.

[1]  Esther Torrego Salcedo On the nature of clitic doubling , 1994 .

[2]  E. C. Hayhow,et al.  Healthcare agenda for the Indian government , 1939, The Indian journal of medical research.

[3]  J. Nichols Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time , 1992 .

[4]  Ian Roberts,et al.  Agreement and Head Movement: Clitics, Incorporation, and Defective Goals , 2010 .

[5]  Margarita Suñer,et al.  The role of agreement in clitic-doubled constructions , 1988 .

[6]  L. Rizzi Null objects in Italian and the theory of 'pro' , 1986 .

[7]  Julie Anne Legate,et al.  Warlpiri : theoretical implications , 2002 .

[8]  Carmen Dobrovic-Sorin Clitic doubling, 'Wh'-movement and quatification in Romanian , 1990 .

[9]  Mürvet Enç The semantics of specificity , 1991 .

[10]  Mary Dalrymple,et al.  Objects and Information Structure , 2011 .

[11]  T. Laczkó On oblique arguments and adjuncts of Hungarian event nominals − A comprehensive LFG account , 2000 .

[12]  Esther Torrego From argumental to non-argumental pronouns: Spanish doubled reflexives , 1995 .

[13]  Katalin É. Kiss,et al.  The syntactic structure of Hungarian , 1992 .

[14]  E. Anagnostopoulou The syntax of ditransitives : evidence from clitics , 2003 .

[15]  Avery D. Andrews,et al.  Unification and morphological blocking , 1990 .

[16]  Oleg Golubitsky,et al.  What is a structural representation , 2001 .

[17]  N. Ruwet En et y : deux clitiques pronominaux antilogophoriques , 1990 .

[18]  Joan Bresnan,et al.  On Topic, Pronoun, and Agreement in Chichewa. , 1985 .

[19]  Martin Everaert,et al.  The Blackwell Companion to Syntax , 2006 .

[20]  T. Biberauer The limits of syntactic variation , 2008 .

[22]  Irene Heim,et al.  The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases : a dissertation , 1982 .

[23]  Frits Beukema,et al.  Clitic phenomena in European languages , 2000 .

[24]  J. Bresnan,et al.  Topic, Pronoun, and Agreement in Chicheŵa@@@Topic, Pronoun, and Agreement in Chichewa , 1987 .

[25]  Mark C. Baker,et al.  The Polysynthesis Parameter , 1995 .

[26]  Hagit Borer,et al.  Parametric Syntax: Case Studies in Semitic and Romance Languages , 1984 .

[27]  E. Anagnostopoulou Clitic Doubling , 2005 .

[28]  Anna Siewierska,et al.  From anaphoric pronoun to grammatical agreement marker: Why objects don't make it. , 1999 .

[29]  Cedric Boeckx,et al.  Aspects of the Syntax of Agreement , 2007 .

[30]  Formal Approaches to Function in Grammar: In honor of Eloise Jelinek , 2003 .

[31]  Norbert Corver,et al.  Organizing Grammar. Linguistic Studies in Honor of Henk van Riemsdijk , 2006 .

[32]  Charles J. Fillmore,et al.  Pragmatically Controlled Zero Anaphora , 1986 .

[33]  K. Kiss Configurationality in Hungarian , 1987 .

[34]  Paula Kempchinsky,et al.  Evolution and revolution in linguistic theory , 1997 .

[35]  Katalin É. Kiss,et al.  Why noun-complement clauses are barriers , 1990 .

[36]  Richard S. Kayne Expletives, datives, and the tension between morphology and syntax , 2008 .

[37]  B. Wald The Development of the Swahili Object Marker: A Study of the Interaction of Syntax and Discourse , 1979 .

[38]  Robert M. Vago,et al.  The Hungarian Language , 1972 .

[39]  Javier Gutiérrez-Rexach,et al.  The Formal Semantics of Clitic Doubling , 1999, J. Semant..

[40]  Guglielmo Cinque,et al.  Types of Ā-dependencies , 1990 .

[41]  I. Kenesei,et al.  Crossing boundaries : advances in the theory of Central and Eastern European languages , 1999 .

[42]  M. Galy,et al.  Papers from the tenth Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society, April 19-21, 1974 , 1974 .

[43]  Lioba J. Moshi,et al.  Object asymmetries in comparative Bantu syntax , 1990 .

[44]  K. Hale On the significance of Eloise Jelinek’s Pronominal Argument Hypothesis , 2003 .

[45]  Talmy Givón,et al.  Discourse and syntax , 1982 .

[46]  Johan Rooryck,et al.  Phrase structure and the lexicon , 1996 .

[47]  Dalina Kallulli,et al.  Direct Object Clitic Doubling in Albanian and Greek , 2000 .

[48]  O. Jaeggli Topics in Romance syntax , 1982 .

[49]  M. D. Dikken,et al.  When Hungarians Agree (to Disagree) — The Fine Art of ‘Phi’ and ‘Art’ , 2006 .

[50]  Donka F. Farkas,et al.  Specificity Distinctions , 2002, J. Semant..

[51]  Cheng-Teh James Huang,et al.  Logical Relations in Chinese and the Theory of Grammar , 1998 .

[52]  Carol Howe Rounds,et al.  Hungarian: An Essential Grammar , 2001 .

[53]  Luis López,et al.  A Derivational Syntax for Information Structure , 2009 .

[54]  Irina Nikolaeva,et al.  Secondary topic as a relation in information structure , 2001 .

[55]  N. Ohashi,et al.  Agreement , 2002 .

[56]  Marianne Mithun,et al.  Pronouns and agreement: The information status of pronominal affixes , 2003 .

[57]  J. Barwise,et al.  Generalized quantifiers and natural language , 1981 .

[58]  J. Rooryck,et al.  CLITIC CONSTRUCTIONS , 2000 .

[59]  Megumi Kameyama,et al.  Zero anaphora: The case of Japanese , 1990 .

[60]  S. J. Keyser Linguistic inquiry monographs , 1976 .

[61]  Open Linguistics Forum,et al.  The Minimalist Parameter: Selected papers from the Open Linguistics Forum, Ottawa, 21–23 March 1997 , 2001 .

[62]  Juan Uriagereka Aspects of the syntax of clitic placement in western romance , 1995 .

[63]  Charles N. Li,et al.  Subject and topic , 1979 .

[64]  Ferenc Havas Objective conjugation and medialisation , 2004 .

[65]  Irina Nikolaeva,et al.  Object Agreement, Grammatical Relations, and Information Structure , 1999 .

[66]  János Gulya Eastern Ostyak chrestomathy , 1966 .

[67]  M. D. Dikken,et al.  On the Structural Representation of Possession and Agreement: the Case of (Anti-)agreement in Hungarian Possessed Nominal Phrases , 1999 .

[68]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .

[69]  M. Nespor,et al.  Grammar in progress , 1990 .

[70]  José Camacho The syntax of NP coordination , 1997 .

[71]  K. Kiss Syntax of Hungarian , 2017 .

[72]  Nicholas Evans Why argument affixes in polysynthetic languages are not pronouns: evidence from Bininj Gun-wok , 1999 .

[73]  J. Bresnan,et al.  Non-configurationality in Australian aboriginal languages , 1996 .

[74]  Mary Dalrymple,et al.  Objects and Information Structure: References , 2011 .

[75]  J. Bresnan Lexical-Functional Syntax , 2000 .

[76]  Huba Bartos,et al.  Object Agreement in Hungarian: A case for Minimalism , 2001 .

[77]  Verb conjugation in Tundra Nenets , 2005 .

[78]  E. Jelinek Empty categories, case, and configurationality , 1984 .