Dopamine D1 activation shortens the duration of phases in stereotyped grooming sequences

[1]  S. McBride,et al.  Altered mesoaccumbens and nigro-striatal dopamine physiology is associated with stereotypy development in a non-rodent species , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  J. Evenden,et al.  The psychopharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VIII: effects of amphetamine, methylphenidate, and other drugs on responding maintained by a fixed consecutive number avoidance schedule , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  J. W. Aldridge,et al.  Sequential super-stereotypy of an instinctive fixed action pattern in hyper-dopaminergic mutant mice: a model of obsessive compulsive disorder and Tourette's , 2005, BMC Biology.

[4]  Ann M Graybiel,et al.  Repetitive Behaviors in Monkeys Are Linked to Specific Striatal Activation Patterns , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  F. Richer,et al.  Tourette syndrome and dopaminergic genes: a family-based association study in the French Canadian founder population , 2004, Molecular Psychiatry.

[6]  W. Meck,et al.  Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  Increased response switching, perseveration and perseverative switching following d-amphetamine in the rat , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[8]  J. Waddington,et al.  Dopaminergic behaviour stereospecifically promoted by the D1 agonist R-SK & F 38393 and selectively blocked by the D1 antagonist SCH 23390 , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[9]  R. Church,et al.  The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[10]  S. Goldstone,et al.  The effects of secobarbital and dextroamphetamine upon time judgment: intersensory factors , 2004, Psychopharmacologia.

[11]  K. Berridge,et al.  Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: control of serial order in a grooming sequence , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  M. Çevik Effects of methamphetamine on duration discrimination. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  M. Nicolelis,et al.  Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  J. Horvitz,et al.  Effects of dopamine antagonists on the timing of two intervals , 2003, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[15]  Christian Gaser,et al.  Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from fMRI , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  L. M. Lieving,et al.  Effects of D-amphetamine in a temporal discrimination procedure: selective changes in timing or rate dependency? , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  C. Buhusi,et al.  Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  A. Dickinson,et al.  The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[19]  R. Palmiter,et al.  Induction of stereotypy in dopamine-deficient mice requires striatal D1 receptor activation , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  P. Lieberman Human Language and Our Reptilian Brain: The Subcortical Bases of Speech, Syntax, and Thought , 2001, Perspectives in biology and medicine.

[22]  D. Brunner,et al.  Pharmacology of Temporal Cognition in Two Mouse Strains , 2001, International Journal of Comparative Psychology.

[23]  K. Berridge,et al.  Intra-Accumbens Amphetamine Increases the Conditioned Incentive Salience of Sucrose Reward: Enhancement of Reward “Wanting” without Enhanced “Liking” or Response Reinforcement , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  K. Berridge,et al.  Super‐stereotypy I: Enhancement of a complex movement sequence by systemic dopamine D1 agonists , 2000, Synapse.

[25]  K. Berridge,et al.  Super‐stereotypy II: Enhancement of a complex movement sequence by intraventricular dopamine D1 agonists , 2000, Synapse.

[26]  S. Mobini,et al.  The effect of d-amphetamine on performance on two operant timing schedules , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[27]  K. Berridge Reward learning: Reinforcement, incentives, and expectations , 2000 .

[28]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Neuronal coding of prediction errors. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  John Evenden,et al.  The Behavior of Spontaneously Hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto Rats Under a Paced Fixed Consecutive Number Schedule of Reinforcement , 1999, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[30]  J. Stewart,et al.  Long-lasting sensitization to the accelerating effects of amphetamine on the speed of an internal clock , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  D. Segal,et al.  Caudate–putamen dopamine and stereotypy response profiles after intravenous and subcutaneous amphetamine , 1999, Synapse.

[32]  D. Segal,et al.  Sensitization of amphetamine-induced stereotyped behaviors during the acute response: role of D1 and D2 dopamine receptors. , 1999, Brain research.

[33]  K. Berridge,et al.  Action sequencing is impaired in D1A‐deficient mutant mice , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  K. Mueller,et al.  Amphetamine-Induced Locomotor Stereotypy in Rats Is Reduced by a D 1 but not a D 2 Antagonist , 1997, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[35]  J. Waddington,et al.  Psychopharmacological Distinction Between Novel Full-Efficacy “D1-like” Dopamine Receptor Agonists , 1997, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[36]  A. Machado Learning the temporal dynamics of behavior. , 1997, Psychological review.

[37]  W H Meck,et al.  The 'internal clocks' of circadian and interval timing. , 1997, Endeavour.

[38]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Risky Theories—The Effects of Variance on Foraging Decisions , 1996 .

[39]  K. Berridge,et al.  Implementation of Action Sequences by a Neostriatal Site: A Lesion Mapping Study of Grooming Syntax , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[41]  K. Berridge,et al.  The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction , 1993, Brain Research Reviews.

[42]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Optimal foraging and timing processes in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris: effect of inter-capture interval , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  P. H. Andersen,et al.  Dopamine receptor agonists: selectivity and dopamine D1 receptor efficacy. , 1990, European journal of pharmacology.

[44]  D. Jermain,et al.  Pharmacotherapy of Obsessive‐Compulsive Disorder , 1990, Pharmacotherapy.

[45]  F. Davis,et al.  Transplanted suprachiasmatic nucleus determines circadian period. , 1990, Science.

[46]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Comparative Fine Structure of Action: Rules of Form and Sequence in the Grooming Patterns of Six Rodent Species , 1990 .

[47]  K. Berridge Substantia nigra 6-OHDA lesions mimic striatopallidal disruption of syntactic grooming chains: A neural systems analysis of sequence control , 1989, Psychobiology.

[48]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Progressive degradation of serial grooming chains by descending decerebration , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  J. Waddington,et al.  The induction of grooming and vacuous chewing by a series of selective D-1 dopamine receptor agonists: two directions of D-1:D-2 interaction. , 1989, European journal of pharmacology.

[50]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing , 1988 .

[51]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.

[52]  D. Eckerman,et al.  Effects of methylphenidate and d-amphetamine on timing in the rat , 1987, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[53]  J. C. Fentress,et al.  Natural syntax rules control action sequence of rats , 1987, Behavioural Brain Research.

[54]  T. Masuhara,et al.  Effects of dopamine on the secretion of glycoproteins from the functional segments of the rat submandibular gland. , 1987, Japanese journal of pharmacology.

[55]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[56]  J C Fentress,et al.  Contextual control of trigeminal sensorimotor function , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[57]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[58]  W. Meck Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[59]  R. Church,et al.  Methamphetamine and time estimation. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[60]  J. Kebabian,et al.  Multiple receptors for dopamine , 1979, Nature.

[61]  A. Cools,et al.  ACTH-induced excessive grooming involves brain dopamine. , 1977, European journal of pharmacology.

[62]  W. Gispen,et al.  The induction of excessive grooming in the rat by intraventricular application of peptides derived from ACTH: structure-activity studies. , 1975, Life sciences.

[63]  E. Ellinwood,et al.  Rating the behavioral effects of amphetamine. , 1974, European journal of pharmacology.

[64]  G. Rylander Psychoses and the punding and choreiform syndromes in addiction to central stimulant drugs. , 1972, Psychiatria, neurologia, neurochirurgia.

[65]  C. P. Richter Sleep and activity: their relation to the 24-hour clock. , 1967, Research publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.