Letter Position Dyslexia

Many word-reading models assume that the early stages of reading involve a separate process of letter position encoding. However, neuropsychological evidence for the existence and selectivity of this function has been rather indirect, coming mainly from position preservation in migrations between words in attentional dyslexia, and from nonselective reading deficits. No pure demonstration of selective impairment of letter position function has yet been made. In this paper two Hebrew-speaking acquired dyslexic patients with occipito-parietal lesions are presented who suffer from a highly selective deficit to letter position encoding. As a result of this deficit, they predominantly make errors of letter migration within words (such as reading “broad” for “board”) in a wide variety of tasks: oral reading, lexical decision, same-different decision, and letter location. The deficit is specific to orthographic material, and is manifested mainly in medial letter positions. The implications of the findings to models of reading and attention are discussed.

[1]  L. Katz,et al.  Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: a multilingual comparison. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  D. Green,et al.  Detecting targets in letter and non-letter arrays. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[3]  Robert B. Katz,et al.  Positional dyslexia , 1989, Brain and Language.

[4]  M. Bender Perceptual interactions. , 1970, Modern trends in neurology.

[5]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia (PALPA) , 1996 .

[6]  K E Patterson,et al.  Neuropsychological approaches to the study of reading. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[7]  M. Jane Riddoch,et al.  Neglect and the peripheral dyslexias , 1990 .

[8]  R. Friedman,et al.  Can Treatment for Pure Alexia Improve Letter-by-Letter Reading Speed without Sacrificing Accuracy? , 1999, Brain and Language.

[9]  D. Neary,et al.  Deep Dyslexia , 1986 .

[10]  A. Kertesz The Western Aphasia Battery , 1982 .

[11]  Richard J. Brown Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .

[12]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[13]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Serial, parallel, or holistic identification of single words in the two visual fields? , 1977 .

[14]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.

[15]  A. Treisman,et al.  Illusory words: the roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to form words. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  M. Mozer,et al.  Perceptual interactions in two-word displays: familiarity and similarity effects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  W Prinzmetal,et al.  Principles of feature integration in visual perception , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  D. Holender,et al.  Analytic approaches to human cognition , 1992 .

[19]  J. Duncan,et al.  Attention and reading: Wholes and parts in shape recognition: A tutorial review. , 1987 .

[20]  M. Mozer Letter migration in word perception. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  K. Patterson What is right with “deep” dyslexic patients? , 1979, Brain and Language.

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Psychological and biological models , 1986 .

[23]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Spatially Determined Deficits in Letter and Word Processing , 1991 .

[24]  James L. McClelland The programmable blackboard model of reading , 1986 .

[25]  D. Bub,et al.  Lexical Constraints on Reading Accuracy in Neglect Dyslexia , 1997 .

[26]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Levels of representation, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect , 1990 .

[27]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter-position coding in random consonant arrays , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience , 1993 .

[29]  E K Warrington,et al.  The dissociation of visuospatial neglect and neglect dyslexia. , 1987, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[30]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Single and multiple component central dyslexic syndromes , 1995 .

[31]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  A rose is a rose or a nose: A deficit in initial letter identification , 1990 .

[32]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  The role of spatial attention in visual word processing. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  A. Baddeley Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition , 1990 .

[34]  Drew H. Abney,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .

[35]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies edited by Ian H. Robertson and John C. Marshall , 1994 .

[36]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Orthographic Neighbours are not all Equal: Evidence using an Identification Technique , 1998, Language and Cognitive Processes.

[37]  Michael Garman,et al.  Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .

[38]  M. Coltheart Disorders of reading and their implications for models of normal reading. , 1981 .

[39]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.

[40]  W. Prinzmetal,et al.  Cognitive and linguistic factors affect visual feature integration , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  T Shallice,et al.  Word-form dyslexia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  J. Marshall,et al.  On psycholinguistic classifications of the acquired dyslexias , 1981 .

[43]  A. Treisman,et al.  Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  F. Velde Effect of orthographic preactivation on letter migration. , 1992 .

[45]  E. Warrington Right Neglect Dyslexia: A Single Case Study , 1991 .

[46]  Susan D. Lima,et al.  The linguistics of literacy , 1992 .

[47]  Mozer,et al.  Early parallel processing in reading: a connectionist approach. Technical report, April-November 1986 , 1986 .

[48]  M Mason,et al.  Recognition time for letters and nonletters: effects of serial position, array size, and processing order. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  Eleanor M. Saffran,et al.  "Attentional Dyslexia" in Alzheimer's Disease: A Case Study , 1996 .

[50]  W. Estes The Locus of Inferential and Perceptual Processes in Letter Identification. , 1975 .

[51]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Laterally presented words: Orthographic analysis and serial, parallel or holistic modes of processing , 1982 .

[52]  William Badecker,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1994 .

[53]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Role of the Graphemic Buffer in Reading , 1996 .

[54]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Graphemic Buffer and attentional mechanisms , 1989, Brain and Language.

[55]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[56]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.

[57]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Attentional dyslexia: The effect of co-occurring deficits , 1993 .

[58]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The possible role of selective attention in acquired dyslexia , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[59]  P. Merikle,et al.  Selective forward masking. , 1972, Canadian journal of psychology.

[60]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[61]  M. Mozer Types and tokens in visual letter perception. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The role of the Graphemic Buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia , 1987, Cognition.

[63]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Different impairments contribute to neglect dyslexia , 1991 .

[64]  D. Bub,et al.  Pure Alexia: Attempted Rehabilitation and Its Implications for Interpretation of the Deficit , 1994, Brain and Language.

[65]  A. W. Ellis Reading, Writing and Dyslexia: A Cognitive Analysis , 1984 .

[66]  A. Ellis,et al.  "Patterns of Paralexia" Revisited: Report of a Case of Visual Dyslexia , 1997 .

[67]  J Grainger,et al.  Neighborhood frequency effects in visual word recognition: A comparison of lexical decision and masked identification latencies , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[68]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology , 2018 .

[69]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  Attentional dyslexia: A single case study , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[71]  J. Marshall,et al.  Patterns of paralexia: a psycholinguistic approach. , 1995, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[72]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[73]  E. Warrington,et al.  A variety of reading disability associated with right hemisphere lesions. , 1995, Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[74]  K. Rayner,et al.  The psychology of reading , 1989 .

[75]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  "Neglect dyslexia" and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords , 1995 .

[76]  J. Riddoch,et al.  Interaction of attentional and lexical processes in neglect dyslexia , 1990 .