Working memory supports listening in noise for persons with hearing impairment.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Brian C J Moore,et al. Speech perception problems of the hearing impaired reflect inability to use temporal fine structure , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[2] Graham Naylor,et al. Linear and nonlinear hearing aid fittings – 2. Patterns of candidature , 2006, International journal of audiology.
[3] Thomas Lunner,et al. Cognitive function in relation to hearing aid use , 2003, International journal of audiology.
[4] Graham Naylor,et al. Benefits from hearing aids in relation to the interaction between the user and the environment , 2003, International journal of audiology.
[5] B Hagerman. Attempts to develop an efficient speech test in fully modulated noise. , 1997, Scandinavian audiology.
[6] Thomas Lunner,et al. Interactions between cognition, compression, and listening conditions: effects on speech-in-noise performance in a two-channel hearing aid. , 2007, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[7] B. Shinn-Cunningham. Object-based auditory and visual attention , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[8] Joshua G. W. Bernstein,et al. Auditory and auditory-visual intelligibility of speech in fluctuating maskers for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[9] T. Houtgast,et al. Factors affecting masking release for speech in modulated noise for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[10] J. Rönnberg. Cognition in the hearing impaired and deaf as a bridge between signal and dialogue: a framework and a model , 2003, International journal of audiology.
[11] Willis F. Overton,et al. Advances in research and theory , 1977 .
[12] Graham Naylor,et al. Long-term signal-to-noise ratio at the input and output of amplitude-compression systems. , 2009, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[13] A. Duquesnoy. Effect of a single interfering noise or speech source upon the binaural sentence intelligibility of aged persons. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[14] Tammo Houtgast,et al. Auditory and nonauditory factors affecting speech reception in noise by older listeners. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[15] T. Lunner,et al. Speech understanding and cognitive spare capacity , 2009 .
[16] Thomas Lunner,et al. Recognition of speech in noise with new hearing instrument compression release settings requires explicit cognitive storage and processing capacity. , 2007, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[17] Stefan Stenfelt,et al. The signal-cognition interface: interactions between degraded auditory signals and cognitive processes. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[18] Björn Hagerman,et al. Speech recognition threshold in slightly and fully modulated noise for hearing-impaired subjects: Umbral de reconocimiento del lenguaje para sujetos hipoacúsicos en medio de ruido parcial o completamente modulado , 2002, International journal of audiology.
[19] A. Baddeley. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[20] Birgitta Larsby,et al. A Swedish version of the Hearing In Noise Test (HINT) for measurement of speech recognition , 2006, International journal of audiology.
[21] L Hickson,et al. Candidature for and delivery of audiological services: special needs of older people , 2003, International journal of audiology.
[22] T. Lunner,et al. Cognition and aided speech recognition in noise: specific role for cognitive factors following nine-week experience with adjusted compression settings in hearing aids. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[23] T. Lunner,et al. Phonological mismatch and explicit cognitive processing in a sample of 102 hearing-aid users , 2008, International journal of audiology.
[24] Richard C. Atkinson,et al. Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes , 1968, Psychology of Learning and Motivation.
[25] R. Plomp,et al. Effects of fluctuating noise and interfering speech on the speech-reception threshold for impaired and normal hearing. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[26] Daniel L. Schacter,et al. The case of K.C.: contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theory , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[27] K E Spens,et al. Cognitive correlates of visual speech understanding in hearing-impaired individuals. , 2001, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.
[28] Brian C J Moore,et al. The Choice of Compression Speed in Hearing Aids: Theoretical and Practical Considerations and the Role of Individual Differences , 2008, Trends in amplification.
[29] Lorienne M Jenstad,et al. Temporal envelope changes of compression and speech rate: combined effects on recognition for older adults. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[30] Thomas Lunner,et al. Cognition and hearing aids. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[31] Pamela E Souza,et al. Effects of Compression on Speech Acoustics, Intelligibility, and Sound Quality , 2002, Trends in amplification.
[32] Thomas Lunner,et al. When cognition kicks in: working memory and speech understanding in noise. , 2010, Noise & health.
[33] Wouter A. Dreschler,et al. ICRA Noises: Artificial Noise Signals with Speech-like Spectral and Temporal Properties for Hearing Instrument Assessment: Ruidos ICRA: Señates de ruido artificial con espectro similar al habla y propiedades temporales para pruebas de instrumentos auditivos , 2001 .
[34] Jingjing Xu,et al. Short and long compression release times: speech understanding, real-world preferences, and association with cognitive ability. , 2010, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[35] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[36] S Arlinger,et al. Evaluation of a cognitive test battery in young and elderly normal-hearing and hearing-impaired persons. , 2001, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[37] M. Daneman,et al. Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[38] L. Humes. The contributions of audibility and cognitive factors to the benefit provided by amplified speech to older adults. , 2007, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.
[39] W. Dreschler,et al. ICRA noises: artificial noise signals with speech-like spectral and temporal properties for hearing instrument assessment. International Collegium for Rehabilitative Audiology. , 2001, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.
[40] P. Carpenter,et al. Individual Differences in Integrating Information between and within Sentences. , 1983 .
[41] T Lunner,et al. A Digital Filterbank Hearing Aid: Three Digital Signal Processing Algorithms‐User Preference and Performance , 1997, Ear and hearing.
[42] S. Arlinger,et al. Visual evoked potentials: relation to adult speechreading and cognitive function. , 1989, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[43] T. Lunner,et al. Cognition counts: A working memory system for ease of language understanding (ELU) , 2008, International journal of audiology.
[44] S. Soli,et al. Development of the Hearing in Noise Test for the measurement of speech reception thresholds in quiet and in noise. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[45] J. Knutson,et al. Psychological Change following 18 Months of Cochlear Implant Use , 1991, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.
[46] Robert H. Logie,et al. Components of fluent reading , 1985 .