“Serial” effects in parallel models of reading

Highlights ► We develop a parallel reading model that includes a visual processing component. ► We examine whether the model can account for “serial” effects found in normal reading. ► The length by lexicality interaction is an emergent property of parallel models with visual processing. ► “Serial” effects in reading can be produced by parallel models. ► Visual processing may be the key to understanding “serial” effects in normal reading.

[1]  D. Plaut,et al.  SD-squared: on the association between semantic dementia and surface dyslexia. , 2007, Psychological review.

[2]  K Patterson,et al.  Letter-by-letter reading: psychological descriptions of a neurological syndrome. , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[3]  M Coltheart,et al.  Reading aloud begins when the computation of phonology is complete. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[5]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Experiments on the spelling-to-sound regularity effect in word recognition , 1978 .

[6]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Visuoperceptual deficits in letter-by-letter reading? , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Using Parallel Distributed Processing Models to Simulate Phonological Dyslexia: The Key Role of Plasticity-related Recovery , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  David C. Plaut,et al.  A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: extension to sequential processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[10]  R. Frackowiak,et al.  Demonstrating the implicit processing of visually presented words and pseudowords. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. , 1987 .

[12]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[13]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud , 1999 .

[14]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[15]  L G Richards,et al.  Recognition thresholds as a function of word length. , 1976, The American journal of psychology.

[16]  S. Petersen,et al.  Effects of Lexicality, Frequency, and Spelling-to-Sound Consistency on the Functional Anatomy of Reading , 1999, Neuron.

[17]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Pure Alexia as a Visual Impairment: A Reconsideration , 1991 .

[18]  T. Nazir,et al.  Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words , 2004, Brain and Language.

[19]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  Martin Kronbichler,et al.  A dual-route perspective on brain activation in response to visual words: Evidence for a length by lexicality interaction in the visual word form area (VWFA) , 2010, NeuroImage.

[21]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Structure and Function in the Lexical System: Insights from Distributed Models of Word Reading and Lexical Decision , 1997 .

[22]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Visual constraints in written word recognition: evidence from the optimal viewing-position effect , 2005 .

[23]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Conjoint and extended neural networks for the computation of speech codes: the neural basis of selective impairment in reading words and pseudowords. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Initial phoneme versus whole-word criterion to initiate pronunciation: Evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. , 1998 .

[25]  Karalyn Patterson,et al.  Letter-by-letter Reading: Psychological Descriptions of a Neurologial Syndrome , 1982 .

[26]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Spatially Determined Deficits in Letter and Word Processing , 1991 .

[27]  E. Warrington Word-form dyslexia , 1995 .

[28]  Tatjana A. Nazir,et al.  Traces of Print Along the Visual Pathway , 2000 .

[29]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Word Length and Orthographic Neighborhood Size Effects in the Left and Right Cerebral Hemispheres , 2002, Brain and Language.

[30]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Effect of onset cluster complexity in speeded naming: A test of rule-based approaches. , 1999 .

[31]  M. Zorzi Serial processing in reading aloud: no challenge for a parallel model. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Effects of word length and frequency on the human event-related potential , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[33]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Prototypicality, distinctiveness, and intercorrelation: Analyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonliving concepts , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  Joseph T Devlin,et al.  The myth of the visual word form area , 2003, NeuroImage.

[35]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical Access and Naming Time. , 1973 .

[36]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.

[37]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[38]  A. Young,et al.  Different methods of lexical access for words presented in the left and right visual hemifields , 1985, Brain and Language.

[39]  Daniel Fiset,et al.  An attempt to simulate letter-by-letter dyslexia in normal readers , 2006, Brain and Language.

[40]  M. Harm Building Large Scale Distributed Semantic Feature Sets with WordNet , 2022 .

[41]  J. Frederiksen,et al.  Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. , 1976 .

[42]  R. Glushko The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .

[43]  Masato Taira,et al.  Reading in a Regular Orthography: An fMRI Study Investigating the Role of Visual Familiarity , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Curt Burgess,et al.  Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence , 1996 .

[45]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on nonword reading , 1998 .

[46]  Stéphane Lehéricy,et al.  The pathophysiology of letter-by-letter reading , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  B. Weekes Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .

[48]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The basis of consistency effects in word naming , 1990 .

[49]  Marlene Behrmann Pure alexia: a nonspatial visual disorder affecting letter activation , 1995 .

[50]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Evaluating Word-Reading Models at the Item Level: Matching the Grain of Theory and Data , 1998 .

[51]  Martin Arguin,et al.  Sequential and parallel letter processing in letter-by-letter dyslexia , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[52]  Padraic Monaghan,et al.  Modeling reading development: Cumulative, incremental learning in a computational model of word naming , 2010 .

[53]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Visual complexity in letter-by-letter reading: Pure alexia is not pure , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Paul M Matthews,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  H. Coslett Evidence for preserved reading in 'pure alexia' , 1995 .

[56]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Neural Correlates of Letter-String Length and Lexicality during Reading in a Regular Orthography , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Ilhan Raman,et al.  Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency: revisited. , 2008 .

[58]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[59]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways , 2008, NeuroImage.

[60]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Modes of word recognition in the left and right cerebral hemispheres , 1988, Brain and Language.

[61]  C. P. Whaley Word–nonword classification time. , 1978 .

[62]  M D Reddix,et al.  Eye movement control during reading: II. Frequency of refixating a word , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[63]  Gordon E Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision , 2001, Vision Research.

[64]  J. Kay Simultaneous form perception and serial letter recognition in a case of letter-by letter reading , 2000 .

[65]  P. Bailey,et al.  Dissociations between serial position and number of letters effects in lateralised visual word recognition , 2005 .

[66]  Arthur M. Jacobs,et al.  On words and their letters , 1991 .

[67]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[68]  M. Zorzi The connectionist dual process (CDP) approach to modelling reading aloud , 2010 .

[69]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. , 2004, Psychological review.

[70]  Daniel N. Bub,et al.  Different modes of word recognition in the left and right visual fields , 1988, Brain and Language.

[71]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Early semantic context integration and lexical access as revealed by event-related brain potentials , 2007, Biological Psychology.

[72]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[73]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999, Psychological review.

[74]  Douglas L. T. Rohde,et al.  An Improved Model of Semantic Similarity Based on Lexical Co-Occurrence , 2005 .

[75]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[76]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[77]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[78]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Psychophysics of reading—XVI. The visual span in normal and low vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[79]  Derek Besner,et al.  The cross-script length effect: further evidence challenging PDP models of reading aloud. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[80]  J. Baron,et al.  Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. , 1976 .

[81]  Peter W. Foltz,et al.  An introduction to latent semantic analysis , 1998 .

[82]  John B. Goodenough,et al.  Contextual correlates of synonymy , 1965, CACM.

[83]  Alan H. Kawamoto Word Length Effects and the Serial vs. Parallel Debate in Connectionist Models of Reading Aloud - eScholarship , 2010 .