The perceptual cognitive processes underpinning skilled performance in volleyball: evidence from eye-movements and verbal reports of thinking involving an in situ representative task.

An extensive body of work has focused on the processes underpinning perceptual-cognitive expertise. The majority of researchers have used film-based simulations to capture superior performance. We combined eye movement recording and verbal reports of thinking to explore the processes underpinning skilled performance in a complex, dynamic, and externally paced representative volleyball task involving in situ data collection. Altogether, 27 female volleyball players performed as centre backcourt defenders in simulated sessions while wearing an eye-tracking device. After each sequence, athletes were questioned concerning their perception of the situation. The visual search strategies employed by the highly-skilled players were more exploratory than those used by skilled players, involving more fixations to a greater number of locations. Highly-skilled participants spent more time fixating on functional spaces between two or more display areas, while the skilled participants fixated on the ball trajectory and specific players. Moreover, highly-skilled players generated more condition concepts with higher levels of sophistication than their skilled counterparts. Findings highlight the value of using representative task designs to capture performance in situ. Key pointsDecision-making in complex sports relies deeply on perceptual-cognitive expertise. In turn, the effect of expertise is highly dependent on the nature and complexity of the task.Nonetheless, most researchers use simple tasks in their research designs, risking not capturing performance in a meaningful way. We proposed to use a live action setting with a complex task design, representative of real world situations.We combined eye movement registration with collection of immediate retrospective verbal reports. Although the two data sets are not directly comparable, they may be used in a complementary manner, providing a deeper and fuller understanding of the processes underpinning superior performance.Highly-skilled players exhibited more exploratory visual search behaviors than their skilled counterparts, performing more fixations into more locations. They also attended more to functional spaces between two or more areas of the display. Furthermore, highly-skilled players produced more condition concepts and with superior level of sophistication, revealing a deeper and more meaningful attunement to the task constraints.

[1]  S. McPherson,et al.  Mapping two new points on the tennis expertise continuum: Tactical skills of adult advanced beginners and entry-level professionals during competition , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[2]  S. McPherson,et al.  How baseball players prepare to bat: tactical knowledge as a mediator of expert performance in baseball. , 2008, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[3]  André Roca,et al.  Identifying the processes underpinning anticipation and decision-making in a dynamic time-constrained task , 2011, Cognitive Processing.

[4]  Keith Davids,et al.  Statistical modelling of gaze behaviour as categorical time series: what you should watch to save soccer penalties , 2011, Cognitive Processing.

[5]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: implications for applied cognitive psychology , 2011 .

[7]  S. Squatrito,et al.  A study of saccadic eye movement dynamics in volleyball: comparison between athletes and non-athletes. , 2010, The Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness.

[8]  A. Williams,et al.  Mechanisms Underpinning Successful Decision Making in Skilled Youth Soccer Players: An Analysis of Visual Search Behaviors , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  David W. Eccles,et al.  The effect of manipulating context-specific information on perceptual-cognitive processes during a simulated anticipation task. , 2011, British journal of psychology.

[10]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Perceiving patterns in dynamic action sequences: Investigating the processes underpinning stimulus recognition and anticipation skill , 2009 .

[11]  Derek T. Y. Mann,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: a meta-analysis. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[12]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological of Expert and Exceptional Performance Capturing the Naturally Occurring Superior Performance of Experts in the Laboratory : toward a Science on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[13]  Paul Ward,et al.  Expertise in basketball modifies perceptual discrimination abilities, underlying cognitive processes, and visual behaviours , 2006 .

[14]  K. Davids,et al.  Ecological Validity, Representative Design, and Correspondence Between Experimental Task Constraints and Behavioral Setting: Comment on Rogers, Kadar, and Costall (2005) , 2007 .

[15]  L. Carlton,et al.  The Use of Anticipatory Visual Cues by Highly Skilled Tennis Players , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  David W. Eccles,et al.  Tracing the process of expertise in a simulated anticipation task , 2009, Ergonomics.

[17]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article EXPERTISE IN TACTILE PATTERN RECOGNITION , 2022 .

[18]  Jerry R. Thomas,et al.  Developing expertise in sport: the relation of knowledge and performance. , 1994 .

[19]  Benoît Lenzen,et al.  Situated analysis of team handball players' decisions: An exploratory study , 2009 .

[20]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. , 2005, Human movement science.

[21]  E. Brunswik Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. , 1955, Psychological review.

[22]  J. Vickers,et al.  Cognitive control in motor expertise , 2004 .

[23]  D. Elliott,et al.  Information processing and constraints-based views of skill acquisition: divergent or complementary? , 2005, Motor control.

[24]  Christopher M. Janelle,et al.  Constraints on the search for visual information in sport , 2004 .

[25]  J. Gréhaigne,et al.  The Teaching and Learning of Decision Making in Team Sports , 2001 .

[26]  S. McPherson,et al.  Expert-Novice Differences in Planning Strategies during Collegiate Singles Tennis Competition , 2000 .

[27]  Armin Kibele,et al.  Non-consciously controlled decision making for fast motor reactions in sports—A priming approach for motor responses to non-consciously perceived movement features , 2006 .

[28]  Eric Laurent,et al.  Extending the rather unnoticed Gibsonian view that ‘perception is cognitive': Development of the enactive approach to perceptual-cognitive expertise. , 2008 .

[29]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Action specificity increases anticipatory performance and the expert advantage in natural interceptive tasks. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[30]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: a general overview. , 2008, Academic emergency medicine : official journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine.

[31]  Arie Selinger,et al.  Arie Selinger's Power Volleyball , 1986 .

[32]  K. Davids,et al.  Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[33]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[34]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed. , 2007 .

[35]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Capturing naturally occurring superior performance in the laboratory: translational research on expert performance. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[36]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Relation of knowledge and performance in boys' tennis: age and expertise. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  V L Patel,et al.  Medical expertise asa function of task difficulty , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[38]  M. P. Moreno,et al.  Application of mentoring through reflection in female setters of the Spanish national volleyball team. A case study. , 2008 .

[39]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Learning from the Experts: Practice Activities of Expert Decision Makers in Sport , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[40]  Christopher M. Janelle,et al.  Visual Attention and Brain Processes That Underlie Expert Performance: Implications for Sport and Military Psychology , 2008 .

[41]  Isabel Mesquita,et al.  Procedural knowledge, decision-making and game performance analysis in Female Volleyball’s attack according to the player’s experience and competitive success , 2011 .

[42]  K. Davids,et al.  Representative task designs for the study of perception and action in sport. , 2009 .

[43]  P. Ward,et al.  Domain Specificity, Task Specificity, and Expert Performance , 2008, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[44]  M. Wilson,et al.  Eye Movements Drive Steering: Reduced Eye Movement Distribution Impairs Steering and Driving Performance , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[45]  J N Vickers,et al.  Gaze behaviors of goaltenders under spatial-temporal constraints. , 2006, Human movement science.

[46]  S. McPherson,et al.  The Influence of Player Experience on Problem Solving during Batting Preparation in Baseball , 1993 .