Learning to Improve: The Biased Processing of Mixed Price Comparisons

[1]  William Samuelson,et al.  Status quo bias in decision making , 1988 .

[2]  Craig A. Anderson,et al.  Abstract and concrete data in the perseverance of social theories: When weak data lead to unshakeable beliefs☆ , 1983 .

[3]  Sherry K. Schneider,et al.  Coping and the Simulation of Events , 1989 .

[4]  K. Markman,et al.  The Impact of Perceived Control on the Imagination of Better and Worse Possible Worlds , 1995 .

[5]  Dan C. Weilbaker,et al.  The Effect of Plausible and Exaggerated Reference Prices on Consumer Perceptions and Price Search , 1988 .

[6]  L. Festinger A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 1957 .

[7]  K. Markman,et al.  Downward Counterfactuals and Motivation: The Wake-Up Call and the Pangloss Effect , 2000 .

[8]  Frank R. Kardes,et al.  Selective hypothesis testing , 1998 .

[9]  J. Landman Regret and Elation Following Action and Inaction , 1987 .

[10]  K. Markman,et al.  The Mental Simulation of Better and Worse Possible Worlds , 1993 .

[11]  Alan Schwartz,et al.  Avoiding Future Regret in Purchase-Timing Decisions , 2001 .

[12]  Carl F. Mela,et al.  The Effect of Discount Frequency and Depth on Consumer Price Judgments , 1999 .

[13]  Stephen J. Hoch,et al.  Consumer Learning: Advertising and the Ambiguity of Product Experience , 1986 .

[14]  James M. Olson,et al.  Outcome Controllability and Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[15]  Donal E. Carlston,et al.  Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. , 1989 .

[16]  W. Swann,et al.  Hypothesis-Testing Processes in Social Interaction , 1978 .

[17]  T. Wills Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology , 1981 .

[18]  S. E. Taylor,et al.  Social comparison activity under threat: downward evaluation and upward contacts. , 1989, Psychological review.

[19]  Jan Fekke Ybema,et al.  Aiming at the top? Upward social comparison of abilities after failure , 1993 .

[20]  Pascal Huguet,et al.  Social comparison choices in the classroom: further evidence for students' upward comparison tendency and its beneficial impact on performance , 2001 .

[21]  J. Greenberg,et al.  Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model , 1987 .

[22]  I. Ritov,et al.  Decision Affect Theory: Emotional Reactions to the Outcomes of Risky Options , 1997 .

[23]  N. Roese,et al.  What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking , 1995 .

[24]  Russell S. Winer,et al.  An Empirical Analysis of Internal and External Reference Prices Using Scanner Data , 1992 .

[25]  Michael Tsiros,et al.  Effect of Regret on Post-choice Valuation: The Case of More Than Two Alternatives. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[26]  K. L. Marsh,et al.  Gaining Control through Counterfactual Thinking , 1999 .

[27]  T. Gilovich,et al.  Biased evaluation and persistence in gambling. , 1983, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  N. Roese The Functional Basis of Counterfactual Thinking , 1994 .

[29]  Joseph W. Alba,et al.  The Influence of Prior Beliefs, Frequency Cues, and Magnitude Cues on Consumers' Perceptions of Comparative Price Data , 1994 .

[30]  R. Collins For Better or Worse: The Impact of Upward Social Comparison on Self-Evaluations , 1996 .

[31]  V. Mittal,et al.  Regret: A Model of its Antecedents and Consequences in Consumer Decision Making , 2000 .

[32]  J. Dyer,et al.  A Generalized Utility Model of Disappointment and Regret Effects on Post-Choice Valuation , 1997 .