Emerging neural specialization of the ventral occipitotemporal cortex to characters through phonological association learning in preschool children

ABSTRACT The ventral occipitotemporal (vOT) cortex serves as a core region for visual processing, and specific areas of this region show preferential activation for various visual categories such as faces and print. The emergence of such functional specialization in the human cortex represents a pivotal developmental process, which provides a basis for targeted and efficient information processing. For example, functional specialization to print in the left vOT is an important prerequisite for fluent reading. However, it remains unclear, which processes initiate the preferential cortical activations to characters arising in the vOT during child development. Using a multimodal neuroimaging approach with preschool children at familial risk for developmental dyslexia, we demonstrate how varying levels of expertise modulate the neural response to single characters, which represent the building blocks of print units. The level of expertise to characters was manipulated firstly through brief training of false‐font speech–sound associations and secondly by comparing characters for which children differed in their level of familiarity and expertise accumulated through abundant exposure in their everyday environment. Neural correlates of character processing were tracked with simultaneous high‐density electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging in a target detection task. We found training performance and expertise‐dependent modulation of the visual event‐related potential around 220ms (N1) and the corresponding vOT activation. Additionally, trained false‐font characters revealed stronger functional connectivity between the left fusiform gyrus (FFG) seed and left superior parietal/lateral occipital cortex regions with higher training performance. In sum, our results demonstrate that learning artificial‐character speech–sound associations enhances activation to trained characters in the vOT and that the magnitude of this activation and the functional connectivity of the left FFG to the parieto‐occipital cortex depends on learning performance. This pattern of results suggests emerging development of the reading network after brief training that parallels network specialization during reading acquisition. HIGHLIGHTSArtificial character‐speech sound training induced preferred N1 and vOT activation.N1 and vOT BOLD tuning depends on training performance in prereaders.Functional connectivity of left FFG and SPL also depends on training performance.Level of expertise to character types modulates the N1 and vOT BOLD activation.Results suggest a phonologically guided N1 and vOT tuning in children.

[1]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing deficits in developmental dyslexia: Beyond the ventral visual stream , 2016, NeuroImage.

[3]  Anthony T Herdman,et al.  Functional Communication Within a Perceptual Network Processing Letters and Pseudoletters , 2011, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[4]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  Maternal history of reading difficulty is associated with reduced language-related gray matter in beginning readers , 2012, NeuroImage.

[5]  Kathryn P. Guy,et al.  Rapid Naming Deficits in Children and Adolescents with Reading Disabilities and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2000, Brain and Language.

[6]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Reading in the brain of children and adults: A meta‐analysis of 40 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[7]  M. Wolf,et al.  Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: a longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. , 1986, Child development.

[8]  C. Price,et al.  The Interactive Account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  B. Shaywitz,et al.  Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[11]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Face perception: domain specific, not process specific. , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  R. Goebel,et al.  Integration of Letters and Speech Sounds in the Human Brain , 2004, Neuron.

[13]  C. Fiebach,et al.  Grey Matter Alterations Co-Localize with Functional Abnormalities in Developmental Dyslexia: An ALE Meta-Analysis , 2012, PloS one.

[14]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Testing computational models of letter perception with item-level event-related potentials , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[15]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Letter processing in the visual system: Different activation patterns for single letters and strings , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[16]  Maya Schneebeli,et al.  Simulating reading acquisition: The link between reading outcome and multimodal brain signatures of letter–speech sound learning in prereaders , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[17]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Jing Zhao,et al.  Fine Neural Tuning for Orthographic Properties of Words Emerges Early in Children Reading Alphabetic Script , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Left cortical specialization for visual letter strings predicts rudimentary knowledge of letter-sound association in preschoolers , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Thomas A Zeffiro,et al.  Development of neural mechanisms for reading , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Tanaka,et al.  A Neural Basis for Expert Object Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.

[24]  E. T. Possing,et al.  Language lateralization in left-handed and ambidextrous people: fMRI data , 2002, Neurology.

[25]  R. Ophoff,et al.  On the relationship between degree of hand-preference and degree of language lateralization , 2015, Brain and Language.

[26]  Clara D. Martin,et al.  Word and object recognition during reading acquisition: MEG evidence , 2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  F. Q. Ribeiro The meta-analysis , 2017, Brazilian journal of otorhinolaryngology.

[28]  Robert Turner,et al.  A Method for Removing Imaging Artifact from Continuous EEG Recorded during Functional MRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[29]  Victoria J. Williams,et al.  Cortical Thickness and Local Gyrification in Children with Developmental Dyslexia , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Evidence for developmental changes in the visual word processing network beyond adolescence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The emergence of the visual word form: Longitudinal evolution of category-specific ventral visual areas during reading acquisition , 2018, PLoS biology.

[32]  Nadine Gaab,et al.  Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  U. Frith,et al.  Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics , 1999 .

[34]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of impaired reading in dyslexic pre-adolescent children , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.

[38]  Olivia Freeman,et al.  Talking points personal outcomes approach: practical guide. , 2012 .

[39]  G. Schulte-Körne,et al.  The time course of reading processes in children with and without dyslexia: an ERP study , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[42]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  An Electrophysiological Study of Print Processing in Kindergarten: The Contribution of the Visual N1 as a Predictor of Reading Outcome , 2013, Developmental neuropsychology.

[43]  U. Frith,et al.  Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Development of Visual Expertise for Reading: Rapid Emergence of Visual Familiarity for an Artificial Script , 2010, Developmental neuropsychology.

[45]  Scott D. Slotnick,et al.  The Visual Word Form Area , 2013 .

[46]  S. Brem,et al.  Neural initialization of audiovisual integration in prereaders at varying risk for developmental dyslexia , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Anthony T. Herdman,et al.  Paying attention to orthography: a visual evoked potential study , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[49]  Iliana I. Karipidis,et al.  Increasing expertise to a novel script modulates the visual N1 ERP in healthy adults , 2018 .

[50]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Tuning of the visual word processing system: Distinct developmental ERP and fMRI effects , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[51]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[52]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  In search of a science-based application: a learning tool for reading acquisition. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[53]  J. Maisog,et al.  A Meta‐analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Dyslexia , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Stephan Bender,et al.  Increased event-related potential latency and amplitude variability in schizophrenia detected through wavelet-based single trial analysis. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[55]  J. Pernier,et al.  ERP Manifestations of Processing Printed Words at Different Psycholinguistic Levels: Time Course and Scalp Distribution , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  Xueting Li,et al.  The Role of Top-Down Task Context in Learning to Perceive Objects , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  The development of print tuning in children with dyslexia: Evidence from longitudinal ERP data supported by fMRI , 2011, NeuroImage.

[58]  Turid Helland,et al.  Brain activation on pre-reading tasks reveals at-risk status for dyslexia in 6-year-old children. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[59]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways , 2008, NeuroImage.

[60]  Assaf Harel,et al.  What is special about expertise? Visual expertise reveals the interactive nature of real-world object recognition , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  Albrecht W. Inhoff Visual word processing. , 1987 .

[62]  Arnold Lohaus,et al.  Marburger Sprachverständnistest für Kinder (MSVK) , 2000 .

[63]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[64]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[65]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Synchronization facilitates removal of MRI artefacts from concurrent EEG recordings and increases usable bandwidth , 2006, NeuroImage.

[66]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Altered responses to tone and phoneme mismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk , 2003, Neuroreport.

[67]  Riitta Salmelin,et al.  Cortical Sequence of Word Perception in Beginning Readers , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  C. Allefeld,et al.  Cortical specialization for attended versus unattended working memory , 2018, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  Jean-François Démonet,et al.  Selective response to letter categorization within the left fusiform gyrus , 2005, NeuroImage.

[70]  Mariano Sigman,et al.  Hierarchical Coding of Letter Strings in the Ventral Stream: Dissecting the Inner Organization of the Visual Word-Form System , 2007, Neuron.

[71]  M. Tarr,et al.  Expertise Training with Novel Objects Leads to Left-Lateralized Facelike Electrophysiological Responses , 2002, Psychological science.

[72]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  An early electrophysiological response associated with expertise in letter perception , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[73]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Prediction of children's reading skills using behavioral, functional, and structural neuroimaging measures. , 2007, Behavioral neuroscience.

[74]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Left hemisphere specialization for word reading potentially causes, rather than results from, a left lateralized bias for high spatial frequency visual information , 2015, Cortex.

[75]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Early development of letter specialization in left fusiform is associated with better word reading and smaller fusiform face area. , 2018, Developmental science.

[76]  Sarah Laszlo,et al.  An event-related potential study of the relationship between N170 lateralization and phonological awareness in developing readers , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[77]  T. Givón,et al.  Brain Plasticity in Learning Visual Words , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[78]  John C Gore,et al.  Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[79]  E. Brannon,et al.  Developmental trajectory of neural specialization for letter and number visual processing. , 2018, Developmental science.

[80]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Development of neural systems for reading. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[81]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  The Development of Cortical Sensitivity to Visual Word Forms , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[82]  Leo Blomert,et al.  The neural signature of orthographic–phonological binding in successful and failing reading development , 2011, NeuroImage.

[83]  M. Tagamets,et al.  A Parametric Approach to Orthographic Processing in the Brain: An fMRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  Anne Bonnefond,et al.  Impaired visual expertise for print in French adults with dyslexia as shown by N170 tuning , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[85]  L. Jost,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of learning to read: print tuning in beginning readers related to word-reading fluency and semantics but not phonology. , 2015, Developmental science.

[86]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  Early identification of dyslexia and the use of computer game-based practice to support reading acquisition , 2007 .

[87]  Thomas T. Liu,et al.  A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI , 2007, NeuroImage.

[88]  G. F. González,et al.  Responsivity to dyslexia training indexed by the N170 amplitude of the brain potential elicited by word reading , 2016, Brain and Cognition.

[89]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Brain-potential analysis of visual word recognition in dyslexics and typically reading children , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[90]  T. Carr The psychology of the four-letter word, plus or minus: Humphreys & Evett's evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[91]  Ulla Richardson,et al.  Print-specific multimodal brain activation in kindergarten improves prediction of reading skills in second grade , 2011, NeuroImage.

[92]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[93]  Urs Maurer,et al.  The development of visual expertise for words: The contribution of electrophysiology , 2007 .

[94]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Emerging Neurophysiological Specialization for Letter Strings , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[95]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[96]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A meta‐analysis of voxel‐based morphometry studies , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[97]  Michael Henry Tessler,et al.  Privileged Functional Connectivity between the Visual Word Form Area and the Language System , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[98]  R Todd Constable,et al.  The neurobiology of adaptive learning in reading: A contrast of different training conditions , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[99]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read , 2006, NeuroImage.

[100]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Neurophysiological signs of rapidly emerging visual expertise for symbol strings , 2005, Neuroreport.

[101]  Laurie S. Glezer,et al.  Evidence for Highly Selective Neuronal Tuning to Whole Words in the “Visual Word Form Area” , 2009, Neuron.

[102]  Orrin Devinsky,et al.  Sequential then Interactive Processing of Letters and Words in the Left Fusiform Gyrus , 2012, Nature Communications.

[103]  Marc Joliot,et al.  Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 children. , 2010, Developmental science.

[104]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[105]  Maximilian Riesenhuber,et al.  Individual Variability in Location Impacts Orthographic Selectivity in the “Visual Word Form Area” , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  H. Lyytinen,et al.  Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter–speech sound correspondences , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[107]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Timing the impact of literacy on visual processing , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[108]  Blair C. Armstrong,et al.  The what, when, where, and how of visual word recognition , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[109]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Children with dyslexia lack multiple specializations along the visual word-form (VWF) system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[110]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[111]  Maximilian Riesenhuber,et al.  Adding Words to the Brain's Visual Dictionary: Novel Word Learning Selectively Sharpens Orthographic Representations in the VWFA , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[112]  Pierre Foussier,et al.  The parametric approach , 2006 .

[113]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure , 2006, NeuroImage.

[114]  Zhong-Lin Lu,et al.  Sensitivity to orthographic familiarity in the occipito-temporal region , 2008, NeuroImage.

[115]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[116]  F. Fazio,et al.  Dyslexia: Cultural Diversity and Biological Unity , 2001, Science.

[117]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[118]  M. Farah,et al.  Neural Specialization for Letter Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[119]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.

[120]  Gui Xue,et al.  Language experience shapes fusiform activation when processing a logographic artificial language: An fMRI training study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[121]  Paul E. Hamburger,et al.  On an automated method , 1966, ACM '66.

[122]  Maria Chang,et al.  Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset , 2011, NeuroImage.

[123]  Guinevere F. Eden,et al.  Attention to single letters activates left extrastriate cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[124]  Donna Coch,et al.  N1 and P2 to words and wordlike stimuli in late elementary school children and adults. , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[125]  Z Kourtzi,et al.  Representation of Perceived Object Shape by the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 2001, Science.

[126]  B. Pennington,et al.  Reliability and Validity of the Adult Reading History Questionnaire , 2000, Journal of learning disabilities.

[127]  Hélène Gros,et al.  Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of the Extrastriate Cortex Response to a Categorically Ambiguous Stimulus Primed by Letters and Familiar Geometric Figures , 2001, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[128]  S. Petersen,et al.  The putative visual word form area is functionally connected to the dorsal attention network. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[129]  D. Brandeis,et al.  Neurophysiology in Preschool Improves Behavioral Prediction of Reading Ability Throughout Primary School , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[130]  Donald Hedeker,et al.  A Practical Guide to Calculating Cohen’s f2, a Measure of Local Effect Size, from PROC MIXED , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[131]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[132]  Shachar Maidenbaum,et al.  A number-form area in the blind , 2015, Nature Communications.

[133]  Amir Amedi,et al.  Origins of the specialization for letters and numbers in ventral occipitotemporal cortex , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[134]  R. Goebel,et al.  Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[135]  Milene Bonte,et al.  Altered patterns of directed connectivity within the reading network of dyslexic children and their relation to reading dysfluency , 2016, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[136]  Eileen M. Napoliello,et al.  Dyslexic children lack word selectivity gradients in occipito-temporal and inferior frontal cortex , 2015, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[137]  O. Churches,et al.  Investigating letter recognition in the brain by varying typeface: An event-related potential study , 2014, Brain and Cognition.

[138]  K. James Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. , 2010, Developmental science.

[139]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  The Neural Substrates of Visual Perceptual Learning of Words: Implications for the Visual Word Form Area Hypothesis , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[140]  M. Seghier,et al.  An anatomical signature for literacy , 2009, Nature.

[141]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[142]  Dustin Scheinost,et al.  Disruption of Functional Networks in Dyslexia: A Whole-Brain, Data-Driven Analysis of Connectivity , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[143]  Martin Kronbichler,et al.  Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults , 2011, NeuroImage.

[144]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Behavioral and Anatomical Consequences of Early versus Late Symbol Training in Macaques , 2012, Neuron.

[145]  M. Kutas,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence for visual perceptual categorization of words and faces within 150 ms. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[146]  Jane E. Joseph,et al.  fMRI correlates of cortical specialization and generalization for letter processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[147]  Carolyn A Denton,et al.  Adequate Versus Inadequate Response to Reading Intervention: An Event-Related Potentials Assessment , 2013, Developmental neuropsychology.

[148]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Age‐related changes in reading systems of dyslexic children , 2007, Annals of neurology.

[149]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[150]  Susan L. Whitfield-Gabrieli,et al.  Conn: A Functional Connectivity Toolbox for Correlated and Anticorrelated Brain Networks , 2012, Brain Connect..

[151]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The unique role of the visual word form area in reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[152]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removal of eye activity artifacts from visual event-related potentials in normal and clinical subjects , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[153]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  A Brain Area for Visual Numerals , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[154]  E. Vogel,et al.  The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[155]  Kuniyoshi L Sakai,et al.  Learning letters in adulthood: direct visualization of cortical plasticity for forming a new link between orthography and phonology. , 2004, Neuron.

[156]  Silvia Brem,et al.  The left occipitotemporal system in reading: Disruption of focal fMRI connectivity to left inferior frontal and inferior parietal language areas in children with dyslexia , 2011, NeuroImage.

[157]  Scott Holland,et al.  Template-O-Matic: A toolbox for creating customized pediatric templates , 2008, NeuroImage.

[158]  F. Wood,et al.  Electrophysiological indicants of black-white discrimination performance for letter and non-letter patterns. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[159]  J. L. Bas,et al.  Pre-orthographic character string processing and parietal cortex: A role for visual attention in reading? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[160]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Visual print tuning deficits in dyslexic adolescents under minimized phonological demands , 2013, NeuroImage.

[161]  Yuliya N. Yoncheva,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for visual words is shaped by attention to sublexical units during initial learning , 2015, Brain and Language.

[162]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Experience-dependent Hemispheric Specialization of Letters and Numbers Is Revealed in Early Visual Processing , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.