Short-term memory for scenes with affective content.

The emotional content of visual images can be parameterized along two dimensions: valence (pleasantness) and arousal (intensity of emotion). In this study we ask how these distinct emotional dimensions affect the short-term memory of human observers viewing a rapid stream of images and trying to remember their content. We show that valence and arousal modulate short-term memory as independent factors. Arousal influences dramatically the average speed of data accumulation in memory: higher arousal results in faster accumulation. Valence has a more interesting effect: while a picture is being viewed, information from positive and neutral scenes accumulates in memory at a constant rate, whereas information from negative scenes is encoded slowly at first, then increasingly faster. We provide evidence showing that neither differences in low-level image properties nor differences in the ability to apprehend the meaning of images at short exposures can account for the observed results, and propose that the effects are specific to the short-term memory mechanism. We interpret this pattern of results to mean that information accumulation in short-term memory is a controlled process, whose gain is modulated by valence and arousal acting as endogenous attentional cues.

[1]  H. Klüver,et al.  "Psychic blindness" and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in Rhesus monkeys. , 1937 .

[2]  C. Osgood,et al.  The Measurement of Meaning , 1958 .

[3]  D. H. Kelly Visual responses to time-dependent stimuli. III. Individual variations. , 1962, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[4]  M. Potter,et al.  Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  I. Biederman,et al.  On the information extracted from a glance at a scene. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Recognition impaired and association intact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[7]  M. Potter Meaning in visual search. , 1975, Science.

[8]  M. Potter Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[9]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[10]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[11]  C Bundesen,et al.  Visual selection from multielement displays: measuring and modeling effects of exposure duration. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Sheldon M. Ross,et al.  Introduction to Probability Models (4th ed.). , 1990 .

[13]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. , 1990, Psychological review.

[14]  D Mumford,et al.  On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. , 1992, Biological cybernetics.

[15]  H. Wilson,et al.  Spatial frequency adaptation and contrast gain control , 1993, Vision Research.

[16]  G R Loftus,et al.  A theory of visual information acquisition and visual memory with special application to intensity-duration trade-offs. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Sheldon M. Ross Introduction to Probability Models. , 1995 .

[18]  S. Thorpe,et al.  Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.

[19]  Anil K. Jain,et al.  On image classification: city images vs. landscapes , 1998, Pattern Recognit..

[20]  James L. McGaugh,et al.  Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  Claus Bundesen,et al.  Single-letter recognition as a function of exposure duration , 1999 .

[22]  Iván Izquierdo,et al.  The Amygdala Is Involved in the Modulation of Long-Term Memory, but Not in Working or Short-Term Memory , 1999, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[23]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Separate mechanisms for short- and long-term memory , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  G. Loftus,et al.  A front end to a theory of picture recognition , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  V. Coltheart Fleeting memories : cognition of brief visual stimuli , 1999 .

[26]  P. Lang,et al.  International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-4) , 1999 .

[27]  S. Nadarajah,et al.  Extreme Value Distributions: Theory and Applications , 2000 .

[28]  A. Anderson,et al.  Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events , 2001, Nature.

[29]  R VanRullen,et al.  Is it a Bird? Is it a Plane? Ultra-Rapid Visual Categorisation of Natural and Artifactual Objects , 2001, Perception.

[30]  P. Fldik,et al.  The Speed of Sight , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Stephan Hamann,et al.  Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  S. Thorpe,et al.  The Time Course of Visual Processing: From Early Perception to Decision-Making , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  B McElree,et al.  Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  P. Perona,et al.  Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Evan Heit,et al.  Perceptual processes in matching and recognition of complex pictures. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  James L McGaugh,et al.  Amygdala Modulation of Memory Consolidation: Interaction with Other Brain Systems , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[37]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Dissociation of Neural Representation of Intensity and Affective Valuation in Human Gustation , 2003, Neuron.

[39]  Bruno A Olshausen,et al.  Timecourse of neural signatures of object recognition. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[40]  C. Koch,et al.  Visual Selective Behavior Can Be Triggered by a Feed-Forward Process , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Statistics of natural image categories , 2003, Network.

[42]  S. Corkin,et al.  Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. , 2003, Emotion.

[43]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Differential effects of emotional arousal in short- and long-term memory in healthy adults , 2003, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[44]  J. Parkinson,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Incentive Motivation and Goal Selection , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Human temporal impulse response speeds up with increased stimulus contrast , 2003, Vision Research.

[46]  D. Mumford,et al.  On the computational architecture of the neocortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[47]  S. Corkin,et al.  Two routes to emotional memory: distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  David Mumford,et al.  On the computational architecture of the neocortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[49]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Attention alters appearance , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  The time-course of categorization of real-life scenes with affective content , 2004 .

[52]  D.,et al.  Visual Responses to Time-Dependent Stimuli . * I . Amplitude Sensitivity , 2004 .

[53]  Wilhelm Wundt,et al.  Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind , 1922 .